Effect of gender and personality characteristics on the speed tendency based on advanced driving assistance system (ADAS) evaluation

Cunshu Pan (Chongqing Jiaotong University, Chongqing, China)
Jin Xu (Chongqing Jiaotong University, Chongqing, China)
Jinghou Fu (Chongqing Jiaotong University, Chongqing, China)

Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles

ISSN: 2399-9802

Article publication date: 9 April 2021

Issue publication date: 26 April 2021

1087

Abstract

Purpose

This study aims to explore the relationship between speed behavior of participants and driving styles on interchange ramps. A spiral interchange in Chongqing was selected as an experimental road to carry out field driving experiment.

Design/methodology/approach

The continuous operating speed during experiment was selected by Mobile Eye, and the driving style was selected via two inventories.

Findings

Different driving behaviors showed great differences in age, driving mileage and driving experience. During driving process, male pursued driving stimulation more, whereas female pursued driving steadiness more. Therefore, driving characteristics of male were more disadvantageous to driving safety than that of female. Except for the large speed difference at the entrance and exit of the ramps, the differences at other positions were small. And the operating speed of male was slightly higher than that of female. The difference between different genders at the ascending end position achieved 4–5 kph, and the difference at other feature points were mostly 1–2 kph. During driving process, risky participants were more likely to pursue driving stimulation, and the poor speed control behavior was reflected in wide range of desired operating speed. Based on the results of analyzing at feature points, melancholy and sanguine participants more tended to take a high operating speed, and the poor speed control behavior was reflected in the most widely desired speed range. The speed control behavior of mixed participants was more cautious.

Originality/value

Advanced driving assistance system combined with two inventories was used to explore difference of speed behavior.

Keywords

Citation

Pan, C., Xu, J. and Fu, J. (2021), "Effect of gender and personality characteristics on the speed tendency based on advanced driving assistance system (ADAS) evaluation", Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 28-37. https://doi.org/10.1108/JICV-04-2020-0003

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2020, Cunshu Pan, Jin Xu and Jinghou Fu.

License

Published in Journal of Intelligent and Connected Vehicles. Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence maybe seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

Driving behavior had always been the focus of research in the field of traffic safety, and different subjects had different perspectives on driving behavior.

Scholars who focused on traffic psychology tended to use different scales to analyze the correlation among the participant's sociodemographic factor, personality, self-reported information and the scale factor, as well as to investigate the relationship between the driving style and the scale factors. Taubman-Ben-Ari and Yehiel (2012); Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky (2016); and Taubman-Ben-Ari et al. (2004) used NEO-five factor inventory (NEO-FFI) and multidimensional driving style inventory (MDSI) to discuss the correlation between participants with different personality characteristics (such as different genders, different ages, different educational levels, different working status) and driving behavior. Based on the information selected by brief sensation seeking scale, Zimbardo time perspective inventory and NEO-FFI, Linkov et al. (2019) combined the operating data of participant on the driving simulator with fixed scene to analyze the relationship between operating speed and responsibility. Bernstein and Calamia (2019) combined the participant's self-reported driving behavior with information selected via different scales and used exploratory factor analysis to reveal the correlation between factors. W. Chu et al. (2019) revealed the relationship among external affective demand, functionality, internal requirement and driving style. Steinbakk et al. (2019) used UPPS-impulsivity scale to discuss the relationship between speed choice behavior and personality traits of different drivers in the work area.

To illustrate the relationship between traffic safety and driving behavior, scholars were more inclined to investigate the various operating data (operating speed, lateral acceleration, pedal force, etc). with theoretical models, driving simulator and field driving experiment to discuss the relationship among the various operating data. Chevalier et al. (2016) investigated the ability of elderly drivers with cognitive decline to control their speed during driving process and revealed the impact of cognitive decline on the speed of older drivers (). Based on driving motivation, turn signal usage, duration, urgency and impact, Wang et al. (2019a) established multilevel mixed-effects linear models to deal with unsafe lane-change phenomenon. Combined with physiological index and braking requirement of driver, Musicant et al. (2019) revealed the relationship between driver’s heart rate and deceleration intensity during driving. Zhang et al. (2019) discussed the relationship between driving space, a kind of vehicle operating space originated from driver space during driving process, and driving emotion, and revealed the influence of vehicle emotion on distance from surrounding vehicles during driving. Based on the field driving experiment, Eboli et al. (2017) divided the participant into safe, unsafe and safe but potentially dangerous according to the average speed, 50th percentile operating speed and 85th percentile operating speed. Chen et al. (2019) established a graphical approach to reveal driver longitudinal acceleration behavior with different personalities. Wang et al. (2019b) used the Gray relation entropy analysis method to analyze the physiological and psychological factors of driving process and revealed the sequence of influencing factors of driving tendency .

In the prior research, in brief, there were few studies combining the field driving experiment and scale. Some of research studies were based on the section velocity and lacked relevant test of continuous operating speed and driving style tested by scale. The objective of this paper was to discuss the difference in speed behavior between different types of participant during driving and the speed behavior characteristics of various types, and the relationship between individual characteristic and operating speed were explored. Different types of participant were classified by the MDSI and temperament-type inventory (TTI) scales.

Methods

The interchange ramp in Nanan District of Chongqing City, China, was selected as the experimental road, and 30 participants with different individual characteristics were selected from the DiDi (a transportation network company). According to usual driving mode, each participant should drive on two selected ramps until the end of mission. The relationship between operating speed and driving behavior in ramp was analyzed via collecting operating speed during driving.

Before started the driving experiment, the detailed driving route should inform to participant. To restore the driving behavior of each participant during the driving process to the greatest extent, experimenters would not guide the driver during the experiment, and the participant would control the vehicle completely according to his usual driving style. Every participant needed to complete two designated questionnaires to distinguish the driving style. To prevent the result of random answer from affecting the style judgment, each questionnaire should be completed under the guidance and supervision of experimenters. After completing the questionnaire, participant listened to the instruction of the experimenter to start the driving task. Each participant's driving task is 2–4 rounds, and each round was required traveling the designated test ramp at least once (completing a travel from start position to end position as a round).

Experimental road and vehicle

Experimental equipment included Mobile Eye and two driving recorders. Mobile Eye was an important part of advanced driving assistance system. Its functions included collision warning, lane departure warning, vehicle speed recording, etc. The way of Mobile Eye collected speed data by connecting the data port of the instrument to the controller area network of the vehicle to obtain the vehicle operating speed transmitted by the on-board ECU in real-time with the acquisition frequency of about 10 Hz, and the speed data of Mobile eye was consistent with that of the instrument panel.

Mobile Eye was used to record the continuous operating speed during the experiment. All external environments during the experiment were collected by the front and rear driving recorders. As the field driving experiment was affected by many uncontrollable factors (congestion, car accidents, etc.), so the result affected by uncontrollable factors would be eliminated during the data processing process to increase data reliability. Hyundai Santa Fe was selected as experimental vehicle.

According to Figure 1, Sujiaba interchange was located in Nanan District of Chongqing, which connected the Caiyuanba Yangtze River Bridge and other major roads. Two ramps on Sujiaba interchange were selected as experimental road, including an ascending ramp and a descending ramp, shown in Figure 1. Ascending ramp was composed of an oval curve (blue area in Figure 1) and a S-shaped curve (green area in Figure 1), and descending ramp consisted of an oval curve. Both of ramps connected Caiyuanba Yangtze River Bridge and Nantong Road.

Multidimensional driving style inventory and temperament-type inventory

Orit Taubman-Ben-Ari et al. designed and validated MDSI in 2004, and it was one of the most widely used driving style scales in 20 years. On the basis of the MDSI, Sun et al. (2014) adapted the scale and verified its reliability and validity and compiled MDSI-C which was more suitable for China's national conditions. This paper used MDSI-C to collect the driving style of participants.

According to TTI, compiled by Zhang and Chen (1985) in 1985, participants were divided into four different temperament types (choleric, sanguine, melancholy, phlegmatic participants) and several mixed temperaments. Liu et al. (2006) defined the characteristics of four temperament types in driving style.

This experiment used above-mentioned scales to divide participants into several descriptions and explored speed trend and behavior characteristics of every description. The trend and characteristics of every description were deeply combined with driving video to analyze and summarize behavior characteristics of driving style.

Participant

Taking into account the driving safety during the experiment, participants were required to have certain driving experience to avoid accidents and casualties during the experiment. According to all the samples provided by DiDi, 30 drivers were selected as participants, and they were asked to finish the MDSI-C and TTI. Based on the results of the scale, participants were classified according to demographic characteristics and driving styles. As a result, the demographic characteristics were mainly based on genders, and the driving style of the participant was mainly based on the results of MDSI-C and TTI.

Result

Analysis of driving styles according to gender

According to gender and personal information, the data selected by the MDSI-C and the TTI were listed by the relationship among different factors, gender and self-reported information, as shown in Tables 1–3.

Means and SDs of factors based on MDSI-C and TTI were listed in Tables 1 and 2, which showed the difference between male and female participants under different factors. The factors with higher score were dissociative, angry and risky, and the driving behaviors related to the three factors were anxious, angry and risky. According to the analysis of driving videos, the dissociative factor during driving was mainly caused by communication with other person, indicating that participant expected to “communicate + driving” model instead of focusing only on driving. The angry factor was mainly caused by improper driving (e.g. jump a queue, frequent lane change, etc). of other vehicles and other vulnerable traffic participants (pedestrians, motorcycles, etc). sudden broken in, and participants mainly manifested by cursing, whistling and impatience. The main reasons for risk factor were high operating speed during driving (in most cases, it is higher than the speed limit) and participant’s overconfidence.

The results of means and SDs of driving style measured by the MDSI-C and self-reported information (age, driving mileage and driving experience) were shown in Table 3. As a result, the mean age of anxious participant was the highest, and the mean age of risk participant was the lowest, and the mean driving mileage was also the highest anxious participant, the lowest risk participant, and anxious participant were often characterized by short driving mileage (<100,000 kilometers) and long driving mileage (>500,000 kilometers). Similarly, anxious participant also showed polarization in mean driving experience.

The means and SDs of four temperament types based on TTI were listed in Table 2. Choleric, sanguine, phlegmatic scores of males were more discrete than those of females, and choleric, sanguine, melancholy mean scores of males were higher than that of females. Based on Tables 1 2 and driving video, female was more inclined to focus on driving (driving stability was higher), male was characterized by irritability, impatience and preference for conversation, which was not conducive to safety.

The 15th and 85th percentile speed were selected as feature percentile speed, and the feature percentile speed curves of the descending and ascending ramps were showed in Figure 2. Male and female were indicated by a solid black line and a black dotted line, respectively. The green area represented the operating speed male > female, and the orange area represented female > male. The column represented the difference between the speed difference according to gender and the minimum operating speed (green: male > female, orange: female > male).

Figure 2(a) and (b) was the 15th operating speed for different genders in the descending and ascending ramps. In Figure 2(a), operating speed of male was generally higher than that of female, and the differences were within 10% (generally at 2–3kph), and the differences of the entrance and exit were 16% and 17%, respectively. In Figure 2(b), due to the good road alignment and line of sight at the entrance, operating speed of female was higher than male between 40 and 160 m, and the differences were lower than 10% (1–4kph). At 180–960m, male ran faster than female, and the differences were within 10% (generally at 1–4kph). 980–1100m, the section closed to the exit, female operating speed higher than male, and the differences were 14% (5kph).

Figure 2(c) and (d) were the 85th operating speed for different genders in the descending and ascending ramps. In Figure 2(c), the difference was 28% (7kph) when entering the ramp, which was the largest difference in entire ramp. The differences between 160 and 740 m were maintained in a small region (generally at 1kph, the max was 2kph). Close to the exit (760–840m), the differences were gradually increased (5%–7%, 3–4kph). In Figure 2(d), the differences were relatively stable and had been maintained at 1–3kph. At exit, the maximum difference reached 19% (7kph).

In the previous research, the research on the operating speed focused on the difference of operating speed of feature points (DSP, DEP, ASP and AEP) and analyzed the operating speed characteristics under different feature positions (Fu and Xu, 2019). The author extracted the feature positions and distinguished them according to the feature percentile positions (15th, 50th and 85th) as the previous research, and the 85th percentile value was selected to analyze difference between genders. The results were summarized in Table 4.

DPS, DEP, ASP and AEP represented decelerating start position, decelerating end position, accelerating start position and accelerating end position, respectively. Owning to ascending ramp that had two different accelerating and decelerating processes, the ramp was divided into two sections according to the different accelerating and decelerating processes.

The feature positions of the descending ramp were 380 m, 520 m and 820 m, respectively. In Figure 2(a), the difference at 380 m was 5% (2 kph), 2% (1 kph) at 820 m and basically same at 520 m. In Figure 2(c), the difference at 380 m was 2% (1 kph), the same at 520 m and 7% (5 kph) at 820 m.

The feature points of AR1 were 300, 480 and 740m, and that of AR2 were 760, 920 and 1020 m. In Figure 2(b), the difference at 300 m was 6% (3 kph), 5% (2kph) at 480 m, 4% (2 kph) at 740 m, 3% (2 kph) at 760 m, 4% (2 kph) at 920 m and 9% (4 kph) at 1020 m. In Figure 2(d), the difference at 300 m was basically same, 5% (2 kph) at 480 m, 1% (1 kph) at 740 m, 2% (1 kph) at 760 m, 5% (2 kph) at 920 m and 3% (2 kph) at 1020 m.

In summary, the difference between different genders was larger (4–5 kph) at the AEP, while difference of other feature positions centralized in 1–2 kph, and the maximum was not more than 3 kph.

Analysis of speed behavior according to multidimensional driving style inventory-C and temperament-type inventory

Analysis of speed behavior according to multidimensional driving style inventory-C

According to the driving styles selected by MDSI-C, the participants were classified according to the driving styles, and the operating speed distribution figures of each style on the descending and ascending ramps were obtained, as shown in Figure 3. The 15th, 50th and 85th percentile positions in Table 4 were marked with different colors (Blue-15th, Orange-50th, Green-85th, Red-DSP, Yellow-DEP/ASP, Black-AEP), as shown in Figure 3 (e.g. Blue and Red-15th DSP, Orange and Black-50th AEP). Among Figure 3, the figures on left side were the descending ramp (a, c, e), and the figures on right side were the ascending ramp (b, d, f).

In Figure 3(a), the operating speed was increased from 0 m to 180 m when entering the ramp, the difference at the entrance reached 30kph, and the differences were remained at 17–21kph. At 200–500m, the operating speed showed a downward trend, and the differences were maintained at 12-20kph. At 300–400m, the change of operating speed was relatively stable (difference: 16–20kph, the minimum speed difference and the maximum speed difference were not more than 2kph). After 520 m, the maximum operating speed was gradually increased and the minimum operating speed was gradually decreased, and the differences were increased (difference: 12–40kph). In Figure 3(b), the operating speed from 0 m to 200 m was decreased, and the differences were gradually decreasing (difference: 41-12kph). From 200 m to 920 m, the differences of most sections (200–500m and 740 −920m) were maintained in a small interval (10–15kph), the maximum and minimum speeds were distributed in 52–64kph and 38–44kph, respectively. From 940 m, the operating speed was decreased, and the difference started to increase gradually (difference: 19–33kph).

In Figure 3(c), the speed from the entrance to 220 m was gradually increased, the maximum speed of 300–440m was gradually decreased and the minimum speed was gradually increased. The maximum and minimum speeds of 480–640m were gradually increased, and the maximum and minimum speeds of 740–800m were gradually decreased. The differences were relatively small at 0–140 m and 460–640m, and the differences at 180–320m and 660–780m were larger, while the maximum difference was lower than 29kph. In Figure 3(d), the maximum and minimum speeds of 0–100m and 120–280m had a same trend. After a period of decline (300–380m), the speed started to increase gradually from 480–740m, the minimum speed remained unchanged, the maximum speed was gradually increased, the maximum speed of 760–860m was decreased, and the minimum speed was showed without change. At 880–1100m, the speed was gradually reduced after a small increased. The speed differences were larger at 100–140m and 760–800m, and the difference from 520 to 760 m was gradually increased, and the remaining differences were around 20kph.

In Figure 3(e), the speed was gradually increased from 0 m to 160 m, the maximum and minimum speeds of 180–640m were relatively stable, and the differences were also maintained at 23–27kph. From 740 m, the difference and operating speed were increased. In Figure 3(f), the maximum speed of 0–160m showed a downward trend, and the minimum speed decreased first and then increased, and the difference at the entrance reached 50kph. The maximum speed of 500–700m was decreased, and the minimum speed was the first to decrease and then increase. The maximum and minimum speeds of 740–860m were the same as the decreased trend. The maximum and minimum speeds of 880–960m were the same as the increased trend, and then declined.

In summary, in the descending ramp, there was a large speed difference at the entrance between the angry and risky participants, and the differences of risky participants were greater. maximum speed when entering the ramp: Risky > Angry > Anxious, the operating speed after the accelerating process of entering the ramp: Risky = Angry > Anxious. There was a significant decelerating trend for angry participants, and the anxious and risky participants were more stable. When driving out of the ramp, there was a speed-up phenomenon for both angry and risky participants, and anxiety participants tended to move at a constant speed or slow down.

In the ascending ramp, there was a large speed difference at the entrance of the angry and risky participants, and the difference of the risky participants were greater. The maximum speed at the entrance: Risky > Angry > Anxious. The range of speed fluctuation during driving: Risky > Anxious > Angry. The risky and anxious participants had a significant speed-up phenomenon at 460–760m, and the angry participants only decreased with the other two types of participants after a small increased in speed. When out of ramp, risky and angry participants tended to speed up first and then slow down, while Anxious participants tended to slow down until leave the ramp.

Based on Table 4 and Figure 3, the maximum and minimum operating speeds and the speed difference of participants at different feature points were summarized and listed in Table 5. The speed differences at the feature points were compared among different participants, and the difference of expected speed among different types of participants at the same position and the relationship between expected speed and performance were discussed.

According to Table 5, in descending ramp, the maximum speed at the DSP: Risky > Angry > Anxious, and the speed difference: Risky > Anxious > Angry. Maximum speed at the DEP: Risky > Anxious > Angry, and the speed difference: Risky > Anxious > Angry (15th percentile: Anxious > Risky > Angry). Speed difference at AEP: Angry > Risky > Anxious.

In the first curve of ascending ramp, the maximum speed of DSP: Risky > Anxious > Angry (85th percentile: Anxious > Risky > Angry), speed difference: Risky > Angry > Anxious. The maximum speed of DEP: Risky > Anxious > Angry, speed difference: Risky > Angry > Anxious. The maximum speed of AEP: Risky > Anxious > Angry, speed difference: Risky > Anxious > Angry. In the second curve of ascending ramp, the maximum speed of DSP: Risky > Angry > Anxious, speed difference: Risky > Anxious > Angry. The maximum speed of DEP: Risky > Anxious > Angry, speed difference: Risky > Anxious > Angry. The maximum speed of AEP: Risky > Angry > Anxious, speed difference: Risky > Angry = Anxious.

According to the above analysis, the risky participants had the maximum operating speed at the feature points than the other two types of participants, and the angry driver had the lowest. Similarly, the speed differences, that was the desired speed interval, of risky participants were higher than the other two types of participants.

Analysis of speed behavior according to temperament-type inventory

Based on the driving factors were selected by the TTI, the participants were divided into 7 categories, and the distribution of the operating speed of each type of participants in the descending and ascending ramps were investigated, as shown in Figure 4.

In Figure 4(a), the operating speed was increased from 0 m (max: 28 kph, min: 11 kph) to 180 m (max: 61 kph, min: 46 kph) when entering the ramp, 200–500m was slowly decreased, and 520–820m was slowly increased. The speed differences between 0 and 440 m were maintained at 14–19kph, the speed differences between 460 and 700 m were maintained at 6-10kph, and the speed differences between 720 and 820 m were gradually increased to 22kph.

In Figure 4(b), as the maximum speed gradually decreased and the minimum speed didn’t change, the difference of 0– 200m was continuously decreasing (44kph to 15 kph), and the speed of 200 m-820m was the process of two first rising and then falling, respectively. It had also grown from small to large then to small. When driving away from the ramp, operating speed was also the trend of rising first and then falling.

In Figure 4(c), the speed of 0–160m was increased. After a small decrease of 180 m-480m, the speed was gradually increased away from the ramp, the maximum and minimum speed differences were respectively at the entrance and exit, and the speed differences of the remaining positions were maintained at 20kph. In Figure 4(d), the maximum and minimum operating speeds of 0–180m were decreasing. After a stable operation for a certain distance, the maximum and minimum operating speeds of 300–760m were the same as the first rising and then decreasing, and the trend of 780–1100m was the same as the decrease-increase- decrease trend. The speed difference reached a maximum (42 kph) at entrance, the 0 m-140m speed differences were maintained at a high level (>30kph), and the remaining positions were remained at 20–25kph.

In Figure 4(e), the running speed of 0 m-140m was increased, and the speed of 160–520m was maintained in a small range (max: 58–62kph, min: 34–38kph). From 540 m, the speed difference showed a different trend, and the differences (> 30kph) increased significantly. In Figure 4(f), the speed differences at the entrance (0–80m) and the middle section of the ramp to the driving ramp (560–1080m) were maintained at 30 kph. The operating speed trend was the same as that of the Sanguine participants, and there was a significant drop-up-drop phenomenon.

In Figure 4(g), the speed of 0–180m showed an upward trend, the maximum speed of 200 m-480m decreased, the minimum speed was maintained at 40kph, the speed differences decreased gradually, the speed of 500–800m showed an upward trend, and the speed difference began to increase gradually at 680 m. In Figure 4(h), 0–140m, 480–600m, 1020–1060m speed differences of three sections >25kph, speed differences of 180–320m and 800–920m <15kph. The maximum and minimum operating speeds had a same trend from 200 m to 1060 m.

Figure 4(i), (k) and (m) were the speed trends of the descending ramp of three mixed participants (sanguine-melancholy, choleric-phlegmatic and melancholy-phlegmatic). 0–180m of mixed participants were on the same trend, and the driving speeds of the two types of sanguine-melancholy and melancholy-phlegmatic participants were the same. The choleric-phlegmatic participants had large speed differences between 360 m and 600 m. The speed differences between the sanguine-melancholy and melancholy-phlegmatic participants were larger when driving away from the ramp. Operating speeds of Sanguine-melancholy participants were declined as it leaved the ramp, and the other two type participants were on the rise.

Figure 4(j), (l) and (n) was the operating speed trends of three mixed participants (sanguine-melancholy, choleric-phlegmatic and melancholy–phlegmatic, respectively). The mixed participants entrance ran at a significantly lower speed than the other four types of participants. The choleric-phlegmatic and melancholy-phlegmatic participants with large differences in speed were at the exit. The speed differences of sanguine-melancholy participants were generally stable and did not have much fluctuation.

According to the data in Table 4 and Figure 4, the maximum and minimum speeds and speed difference of different types at different feature points were summarized in Table 6.

According to Table 6, in the descending ramp, maximum speed at DSP: phlegmatic > sanguine > melancholy > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > choleric-phlegmatic. Maximum speed at DEP: melancholy > sanguine > choleric-phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > sanguine > choleric-phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic. Maximum speed at AEP: sanguine > choleric-phlegmatic > melancholy > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic.

In the first curve of ascending ramp, maximum speed at DSP: choleric > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > melancholy- phlegmatic, speed difference: sanguine > choleric> melancholy > … > melancholy-phlegmatic. Maximum speed at DEP: sanguine = melancholy = phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > phlegmatic > sanguine > … > melancholy-phlegmatic. Maximum speed at AEP: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > sanguine-melancholy.

In the second curve of ascending ramp, maximum speed at DSP: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > melancholy-phlegmatic, speed difference: melancholy > sanguine > phlegmatic > … > sanguine-melancholy.

In summary, the melancholy and sanguine participants were the two fastest types for speed, followed by the phlegmatic participants and the choleric-phlegmatic participants, and the last were the melancholy-phlegmatic participants. The biggest differences were also the melancholy participants and the sanguine participants, followed by the phlegmatic participants, the smallest being the melancholy-phlegmatic participants.

Discussion

This paper classified the participants according to the data selected by MDSI-C and TTI. Meanwhile, the speed behavior characteristics of different types of participants during driving process and the difference between characteristics and descriptions of participants in operating speed were discussed. In the past research, some conclusions of MDSI-based research studies pointed out that male reckless and angry driving style was more obvious and prominent than female. Therefore, female was more anxious and cautious during driving (Taubman-Ben-Ari and Yehiel, 2012; Taubman-Ben-Ari and Skvirsky, 2016; Taubman-Ben-Ari et al., 2004). Based on the results of participants with different characteristics in China, male was more irritated during driving and more stimulating by high speed. On the contrary, female tended to be more stable in driving. The risky driving behavior that pursued driving pleasure mainly existed in participants with young age, low driving mileage and low driving experience. Angry driving behavior mainly existed in middle-aged participants with considerable driving experience. According to the analysis results, female on some sections drove faster than the male, but it was generally believed that in most cases, the speed of male was higher than that of female.

Based on the operating speed of different types of participants and the characteristics of each type, it was found that there were some differences between the type characteristics of participants and the speed behavior. In the ascending ramp, anxious participants tended to slow down from the ramp, and risky and angry participants tended to speed up and then slow down when they leaved the ramp. However, the driving aggression of angry participants were not manifested in speed behavior, but only in physical behavior (cursing, whistling and impatient).

According to Liu et al. (2006), the definition of different types was founded that choleric participants were not prominent in operating speed. Sanguine participants and melancholy participants were more inclined to pursue high speed, and there were certain differences with the definition. The reason may be that the professional driver would weaken the influence of personality on driving behavior during driving to ensure safety.

Conclusion

On the designated interchange, from the perspective of driving safety, the field driving experiment was held to discuss the operating speed of different participants and analyze the difference among different participants speed and the different characters in driving with the parameters such as typical percentile speed and distance. The continuous operating speed during driving was selected by Mobile Eye, and the driving style was selected by the MDSI-C and TTI. The main findings were as follows:

  • Older participants were more likely to be Anxious, and driving anxiety was more polarized in driving age and driving mileage. The Risky driving behavior was characterized by low age, low mileage and low experience. The Angry driving behavior was characterized by middle-aged drivers with certain driving experience.

  • During driving, male was more motivated to drive, and female was more likely to pursue driving stability. Moreover, male traits of driving (prone to anger, irritability, tended to have conversation, etc). were more detrimental to driving safety than female.

  • In descending and ascending ramps, except for the large speed differences at the entrance and exit of ramps, the differences at other positions were small. In addition, the operating speed of male was slightly higher than female.

  • The differences between different genders at the ascending terminal were 4-5 kph, and the difference of other feature points were mostly 1–2 kph.

  • The Risky participants had higher requirements for speed than the other two types, Anxious participants tended to shift speed and had poor speed control. However, the aggressiveness of the Angry driver was not reflected in the speed.

  • Melancholy and sanguine participants were more inclined to operate at higher speed, and the poor speed control was reflected in the most widely desired speed range. Mixed participant speed control was more cautious.

Figures

Sujiaba interchange

Figure 1

Sujiaba interchange

Speed behavior of participants according to gender

Figure 2

Speed behavior of participants according to gender

Speed behavior of participants according to MDSI-C factors

Figure 3

Speed behavior of participants according to MDSI-C factors

Speed behavior of participants according to TTI factors

Figure 4

Speed behavior of participants according to TTI factors

MDSI-C Factors according to gender

Factors
Gender
Dissociative Anxious Angry High-velocity Risky
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Male(n = 21) 2.74 0.84 1.68 0.68 3.06 1.19 1.59 0.94 3.49 1.09
Female(n = 9) 2.73 0.91 1.67 0.77 2.58 0.72 1.26 0.4 3.07 0.74

TTI Factors according to gender

Factors
Gender
Choleric Sanguine Melancholy Phlegmatic
Max Min Mean SD Max Min Mean SD Max Min Mean SD Max Min Mean SD
Male(n = 21) 16 −8 3.67 5.45 21 −5 5.90 5.87 19 −10 3 7.13 15 −2 4.81 4.33
Female(n = 9) 6 −4 0.89 3.38 12 −5 4.44 4.95 12 −11 0.78 7.52 11 3 6.33 2.40

MDSI-C Driving style according to self-reported information

Age (years) Driving mileage (105 km) Driving experience (years)
Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Risky 32.86 6.85 14.29 10.66 8.24 4.51
Anxious 40.50 7.70 32.50 23.05 11.75 7.22
Angry 34.60 5.54 20.00 5.48 12.40 4.45

Feature points of 15th, 50th, 85th percentile distance

(R)DSP (m) (Y)DEP/ASP (m) (B)AEP (m)
AR 1 AR 2 DR AR 1 AR 2 DR AR 1 AR 2 DR
(B)15th 0 680 180 360 820 420 600 940 600
(O)50th 60 720 200 420 860 480 720 980 680
(G)85th 300 760 380 480 920 520 740 1020 820
Note:

AR1 – oval curve (first curve) of ascending ramp, AR2 – S-shaped curve (second curve) of ascending ramp, DR – descending ramp

Speed of feature points according to MDSI-C driving styles

Speed of DR (kph) Speed of AR 1 (kph) Speed of AR 2 (kph)
Angry Anxious Risky Angry Anxious Risky Angry Anxious Risky
Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis Max Min Dis
DSP 15th 64 46 17 59 37 22 65 37 27 74 33 41 65 36 29 84 33 51 63 47 15 67 40 27 71 43 28
50th 63 46 17 61 35 26 63 37 26 70 30 39 58 37 21 80 27 53 63 48 15 68 40 28 72 37 35
85th 59 39 20 58 37 21 62 37 25 56 41 15 61 35 26 60 39 21 60 47 12 69 31 38 72 35 37
DEP
/ASP
15th 54 38 16 58 34 24 58 37 22 52 38 14 57 30 28 56 34 22 53 40 16 64 33 31 60 27 32
50th 51 38 13 54 36 18 57 32 25 52 40 13 52 32 20 56 30 26 52 42 15 56 33 23 59 27 32
85th 52 38 14 56 40 16 59 33 26 53 42 12 55 32 23 58 33 25 54 40 14 56 34 21 63 30 33
AEP 15th 59 27 32 61 43 18 62 38 24 60 44 16 62 41 21 71 38 33 57 39 18 56 38 18 67 29 39
50th 57 31 26 62 35 27 59 40 19 63 48 15 48 40 28 72 37 35 41 38 18 57 39 18 68 26 43
85th 66 26 40 59 35 24 66 35 31 62 48 14 69 36 32 74 35 39 60 32 24 53 29 24 62 25 37

Speed of feature points according to TTI factors

Speed of DSP (kph) Speed of DEP/ASP (kph) Speed of AEP (kph)
15th 50th 85th 15th 50th 85th 15th 50th 85th
DR Choleric Max 62 60 58 54 49 50 54 58 66
Min 46 43 39 38 42 43 46 50 44
Dis 16 18 19 16 7 7 8 8 22
Sanguine Max 62 62 59 58 54 56 61 62 66
Min 38 39 38 37 35 33 39 40 43
Dis 24 23 21 21 19 23 22 22 23
Melancholy Max 62 60 59 56 57 59 62 59 62
Min 37 35 37 34 36 38 27 31 26
Dis 25 25 22 22 21 21 34 28 36
Phlegmatic Max 65 63 62 58 51 53 59 58 66
Min 45 45 41 40 41 41 42 43 37
Dis 20 18 21 18 10 12 17 15 29
Sanguine-Melancholy Max 59 60 55 54 50 53 56 57 55
Min 45 49 44 44 38 39 46 45 35
Dis 14 11 11 10 12 14 10 11 20
Choleric-Phlegmatic Max 56 53 58 58 53 56 61 59 64
Min 47 48 43 42 33 37 46 47 41
Dis 9 5 15 16 20 19 16 12 24
Melancholy-Phlegmatic Max 53 51 48 46 42 47 51 50 59
Min 38 37 37 37 32 37 42 41 41
Dis 15 14 11 9 10 9 9 9 18
AR 1 Choleric Max 84 80 55 51 52 56 59 61 60
Min 39 40 40 37 35 39 41 43 44
Dis 45 40 15 13 17 17 19 18 16
Sanguine Max 76 68 61 57 52 55 62 68 69
Min 34 27 35 31 34 34 38 47 46
Dis 42 41 26 26 18 21 24 21 22
Melancholy Max 74 66 59 55 55 56 71 72 74
Min 33 30 42 30 32 32 41 40 36
Dis 41 36 17 26 23 24 30 33 37
Phlegmatic Max 73 70 59 55 56 58 65 66 66
Min 33 33 46 34 30 33 40 48 47
Dis 40 36 13 21 25 25 25 19 18
Sanguine-Melancholy Max 60 58 59 56 56 56 58 62 62
Min 44 48 48 44 42 40 50 53 50
Dis 16 10 10 12 14 16 8 9 12
Choleric-Phlegmatic Max 62 57 56 54 51 52 56 59 57
Min 43 41 46 42 40 41 39 44 45
Dis 19 16 10 12 11 11 17 15 12
Melancholy-Phlegmatic Max 65 57 44 41 39 43 47 51 52
Min 46 49 43 38 33 38 45 37 35
Dis 19 8 1 3 7 4 2 14 17
AR 2 Choleric Max 61 61 58 49 48 53 55 57 54
Min 42 43 43 40 42 43 44 39 29
Dis 19 18 16 9 6 10 11 18 24
Sanguine Max 67 68 69 64 57 62 64 63 61
Min 46 47 45 38 38 34 39 37 32
Dis 21 21 24 26 19 28 25 26 30
Melancholy Max 71 72 72 60 59 62 64 66 62
Min 40 40 31 33 33 34 35 30 25
Dis 31 33 42 27 27 27 29 35 37
Phlegmatic Max 65 66 64 55 52 54 57 61 60
Min 46 48 47 42 43 40 42 40 36
Dis 19 19 17 13 9 14 16 21 25
Sanguine-Melancholy Max 60 62 62 54 50 53 54 57 55
Min 52 53 47 40 35 40 40 41 38
Dis 8 9 14 13 15 13 14 16 17
Choleric-Phlegmatic Max 57 59 58 55 55 63 67 68 56
Min 43 44 43 37 43 42 41 38 36
Dis 14 15 15 18 13 21 26 30 20
Melancholy-Phlegmatic Max 48 51 52 44 44 46 48 52 51
Min 43 37 35 27 27 30 29 26 25
Dis 6 14 17 17 17 16 19 26 26

References

Bernstein, J.P.K. and Calamia, M. (2019), “Dimensions of driving-related emotions and behaviors: an exploratory factor analysis of common self-report measures”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 124, pp. 85-91.

Chen, C., Zhao, X., Zhang, Y., et al. (2019), “A graphical modeling method for individual driving behavior and its application in driving safety analysis using GPS data”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 63, pp. 118-134.

Chevalier, A., Coxon, K., Rogers, K., et al. (2016), “A longitudinal investigation of the predictors of older drivers’ speeding behavior”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 93, pp. 41-47.

Chu, W., Wu, C., Atombo, C., et al. (2019), “Traffic climate, driver behaviour, and accidents involvement in China”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 122, pp. 119-126.

Eboli, L., Guido, G., Mazzulla, G., et al. (2017), “Investigating car users’ driving behaviour through speed analysis”, Promet - Traffic&Transportation, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 193-202.

Fu, J. and Xu, J. (2019), “Analysis on the speed behavior of the helix bridge based on naturalistic driving patterns”, In: 19th COTA International Conference of Transportation Professionals. Nanjing: CICTP, pp. 3604-3615.

Linkov, V., Zaoral, A., Řezáč, P., et al. (2019), “Personality and professional drivers’ driving behavior”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 60, pp. 105-110.

Liu, J., Tian, T., Rong, J., et al. (2006), “Initial research on relationship between drivers’ temperament and travel speed”, Journal of Beijing University of Technology, Vol. 32, pp. 27-32.

Musicant, O., Laufer, I. and Botzer, A. (2019), “Changes in physiological indices and deceleration behaviour as functions of braking demands and driver's physiological cluster”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 62, pp. 406-415.

Steinbakk, R.T., Ulleberg, P., Sagberg, F., et al. (2019), “Speed preferences in work zones: the combined effect of visible roadwork activity, personality traits, attitudes, risk perception and driving style”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 62, pp. 390-405.

Sun, L., Yang, C. and Chang, R. (2014), “Revision and preliminary application of multidimensional”, Chinese Journal of Ergonomics, Vol. 20, pp. 6-9.

Taubman-Ben-Ari, O. and Yehiel, D. (2012), “Driving styles and their associations with personality and motivation”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 45 No. 3, pp. 416-422.

Taubman-Ben-Ari, O. and Skvirsky, V. (2016), “The multidimensional driving style inventory a decade later: review of the literature and re-evaluation of the scale”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 93, pp. 179-188.

Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., Mikulincer, M. and Gillath, O. (2004), “The multidimensional driving style inventory–scale construct and validation”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 36 No. 3, pp. 323-332.

Wang, X., Liu, Y., Wang, J., et al. (2019a), “Study on influencing factors selection of driver’s propensity”, Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, Vol. 66, pp. 35-48.

Wang, X., Yang, M. and Hurwitz, D. (2019b), “Analysis of cut-in behavior based on naturalistic driving data”, Accident Analysis & Prevention, Vol. 124, pp. 127-137.

Zhang, T. and Chen, H. (1985), “Report on the preparation of temperament scale and its preliminary application”, Journal of Shanxi University (Philosophy and Social Science Edition), Vol. 1 No. 4, pp. 73-77.

Zhang, Q., Ge, Y., Qu, W., et al. (2019), “Effects of anger and collision history on driver space preference”, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, Vol. 63, pp. 108-117.

Acknowledgements

This paper was supported by NSFC (approval number: 51678099).

Corresponding author

Jinghou Fu can be contacted at: 383817226@qq.com

Related articles