A systematic review on well-being and ill-being in working contexts: contributions of self-determination theory

Paula Martins Nunes (Faculty of Economics, CEF.UP, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal)
Teresa Proença (Faculty of Economics, CEF.UP, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal)
Mauro Enrique Carozzo-Todaro (Center of Technological Sciences, Maranhão State University (UEMA), São Luís, Brazil)

Personnel Review

ISSN: 0048-3486

Article publication date: 28 February 2023

Issue publication date: 26 February 2024

4122

Abstract

Purpose

No systematic review has previously been dedicated to comprehensively investigate predictors of well-being and ill-being in working contexts. Empirical studies have vastly associated well-being as the result of autonomous motivation and basic psychological needs satisfaction, while frustration results in ill-being. The purpose of this study is to integrate the variables identified in empirical studies associated with the occurrence of the phenomena, individual/organizational features and consequences associated with workers' well-being/ill-being.

Design/methodology/approach

This systematic review includes 44 empirical studies published up to February 2021. Findings are summarized based on quantitative analysis of the evidence.

Findings

Results reinforce the role of self-determined motivation and needs satisfaction in promoting well-being, while amotivation and needs frustration led to ill-being. Besides, they indicate that ill-being can both lead to negative consequences and diminish positive work outcomes. Findings also revealed that: integrated motivation does not seem to be empirically distinct from intrinsic and identified motivation in promoting well-being; introjected motivated behaviors may be less harmful to psychological health than externally oriented ones; the relationship between external motivation and well-being/ill-being requires prospective investigations; and amotivation seems to have a detrimental effect in workers' psychological health.

Practical implications

Results provide practical information for HRM practitioners to design work environments and practices that promote employees' psychological health.

Originality/value

An unprecedented framework that aggregates empirical findings regarding the antecedents, predictors and consequences of ill-being/well-being in working contexts is presented.

Keywords

Citation

Nunes, P.M., Proença, T. and Carozzo-Todaro, M.E. (2024), "A systematic review on well-being and ill-being in working contexts: contributions of self-determination theory", Personnel Review, Vol. 53 No. 2, pp. 375-419. https://doi.org/10.1108/PR-11-2021-0812

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023, Paula Martins Nunes, Teresa Proença and Mauro Enrique Carozzo-Todaro

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

The experience of happiness has puzzled philosophers, psychologists and other scholars for centuries (Diener, 1994). Numerous theoretical and empirical studies have been conducted to understand the nature and origins of this universally pursued feeling; while others have endeavored to create instruments to measure and predict the occurrence of such experience in the most diverse contexts and domains of human experience.

Although the terms “happiness” and “well-being” are usually employed synonymously in scientific literature (Diener and Ryan, 2009; Fisher, 2010; Jayawickreme et al., 2012; Ryan and Deci, 2001), happiness is a concept that designates the preponderance of pleasure over negative emotions (Diener et al., 2018; Diener and Ryan, 2009; Ryan and Deci, 2001), while well-being is conceptually broader and encompasses both the ideas of short-term pleasure (i.e. happiness) and long-term personal growth (Ryan and Deci, 2001). In this sense, due to the ambiguity associated with the terms, Diener (2009) has advocated the use of “subjective well-being” in scientific literature to refer to the positive aspect of human functioning. On the other hand, the term ill-being is usually associated with negative emotions and can be inappropriately considered a phenomenon inversely proportional to well-being (Ryff and Singer, 2006).

The concept of well-being and ill-being as opposing phenomena was initially questioned by Bradburn and Caplovitz (1965), who suggested that positive and negative feelings are independent, making it possible for a person to feel “very happy” and “very depressed” simultaneously; these findings were supported by Diener and Emmons (1984), who demonstrated that such events are also independent in terms of how much people feel in their lives over longer time periods.

For Headey et al. (1984), well-being and ill-being present themselves as separate constructs and have different correlates and causes. The authors went further by constructing a model to integrate variables that were found to influence such experiences (Headey et al., 1985), indicating that measures to prevent or mitigate ill-being should be different from those that seek to enhance well-being.

In a more psychological approach, Huta and Hawley (2008) found that well-being and ill-being can be influenced by one's psychological strengths and vulnerabilities, respectively. By showing that strengths can prevent harmful effects of vulnerabilities, while vulnerabilities do not diminish the benefits of strengths, the authors reinforce the idea of different causes for well-being and ill-being.

From the early studies of Bradburn and Caplovitz (1965) to date, numerous studies with distinct approaches and theoretical frameworks have been conducted to deepen the understanding about the determinants that underlie and differentiate both the positive and negative aspects of human functioning, that is the experience of well-being and ill-being. However, Diener et al. (1999) argue that most studies addressing the topic were limited to investigating individual resources and demographic factors associated with the experience. Meanwhile, in order to investigate and analyze the underlying psychological processes through which these factors act on individuals to induce the experience of well-being and/or ill-being, an increasing number of researchers have successfully resorted to self-determination theory (SDT) and the concepts of basic needs and motivation.

Theoretical background: self-determination theory

SDT has been developed gradually over the last four decades and is now considered a universal macro-theory that investigates human motivation, personality development and well-being with a continuing concern regarding the conditions that promote or frustrate human competencies and self-determined behaviors in multiple contexts and domains of life (Gagné and Deci, 2014; Ryan, 2009; Ryan and Deci, 2000b).

According to SDT, individuals have three universal and inherent basic psychological needs (BPN): autonomy (i.e. a full sense of willingness and freedom), competence (i.e. a sense of being efficacious) and relatedness (i.e. a sense of being effectively connected with others) (Ryan and Deci, 2019; Van den Broeck et al., 2010). Although the strength of each need may vary individually, when BPN are adequately satisfied by environmental conditions, individuals experience psychological growth, internalization and well-being (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Deci and Ryan, 2008b; Ryan, 2009; Van den Broeck et al., 2016).

Given the importance and popularity of BPN in organizational studies, much of Ryan and Deci's current research has focused on the association between BPN satisfaction (SBPN) and well-being, through intrinsic motivation - inherent willingness to perform an activity despite the possibility of rewards (Deci et al., 2017; Deci and Ryan, 1985; Ryan and Deci, 2000b). It is also known that autonomous motivated behaviors, rather than controlled ones – oriented by external factors – have a positive effect on well-being, due to greater SBPN (Deci and Ryan, 2000).

In working contexts, the positive association between SBPN and employees' well-being has been well established empirically (La Guardia et al., 2000; Nie et al., 2015; Reis et al., 2000; Sheldon and Elliot, 1999; Sheldon et al., 1996; Williams et al., 2014), indicating that adequate working conditions can lead to SBPN and consequently promote motivation and well-being. Measures of well-being in SDT research are usually operationalized based on hedonic and eudemonic perspectives (Deci and Ryan, 2008a; Huta, 2017): while the hedonic approach focuses on the experience of short-term happiness and pleasure (Lent, 2004; Ryan and Deci, 2001), eudemonic perspective highlights the long-term pursue of virtue and self-actualization (Ryan and Deci, 2001).

Meanwhile, if the experience of well-being is the result of favorable working conditions and SBPN, when the working environment fails to provide satisfaction or promotes the frustration of BNP (FBPN) (i.e. when individuals are prevented to satisfy basic needs), employees may suffer a decrease in well-being, or even experience a state of ill-being (Fernet et al., 2012b; Gillet et al., 2012; Olafsen et al., 2016; Trépanier et al., 2013), which is briefly defined as “subjective vitality, emotional and physical exhaustion” (Adie et al., 2012, p. 52).

SDT researchers have approached ill-being phenomenon in the form of work related stress (Olafsen et al., 2016), somatic symptom burden (Olafsen et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2014), burnout (Bartholomew et al., 2011; Cresswell and Eklund, 2005; Fernet et al., 2010, 2012a; Huyghebaert et al., 2018; Lonsdale et al., 2009; Trépanier et al., 2013), emotional exhaustion (Richer et al., 2002), eating disorder, depression, negative affect and physical symptoms (Bartholomew et al., 2011). However, it is noteworthy that the negative manifestations associated with FBPN have received less attention from researchers than the benefits associated with SBPN (Deci et al., 2017; Van den Broeck et al., 2021).

As noted previously, SDT argues that the concept of BPN can account for both the occurrence of well-being and ill-being, through the mechanisms of SBPN/FBPN, respectively (Ryan and Deci, 2000a). However, only recent studies have empirically demonstrated that SBPN and FBPN are not inverse, but separate and distinct constructs with different consequences: FBPN does not equate to low levels of SBPN, instead, individuals experience FBPNS when, due to adverse environmental conditions, they are prevented from satisfying each of one the three basic needs; in addition, while SBPN can predict well-being, FBPN has been empirically associated with negative subjective outcomes in different contexts of human experience (Bartholomew et al., 2011; Cordeiro et al., 2016; Longo et al., 2016, 2018; Van den Broeck et al., 2016). Nevertheless, due to the scarcity of research examining unsuccessful motivational processes and their consequences on employees and organizations (Deci et al., 2017), FBPN has only gained some prominence in recent years among SDT researchers.

In this sense, given recent empirical evidence concerning the effect of FBPN on ill-being and despite SDT's continuous progress in uncovering and differentiating the underlying mechanisms that result in well-being and ill-being among employees, this paper is the first review to summarize and systematize the results of empirical studies on the subject conducted in working contexts. Therefore, the main purpose of this study is to review and integrate variables identified in empirical studies associated with the occurrence of well-being and ill-being in the workplace. Furthermore, this review aims to expand the analysis regarding the occurrence of such phenomena by identifying individual/organizational features and consequences empirically associated with workers' psychological health, providing recommendations and future research directions to address gaps identified in the literature.

Method

To attain the purpose of the study, the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines were used to standardize, review and report empirical results systematically (Moher et al., 2009). Based on a guideline checklist, PRISMA constitutes a powerful tool for carrying out literature reviews, since they contribute both to the methodological quality of the review process and to its subsequent replicability (Arya et al., 2021; Pussegoda et al., 2017).

PRISMA guidelines outline four sequential processes - identification, screening, eligibility and inclusion - which were strictly followed in this review (see Figure 1).

Data collection

A systematic literature search was conducted in February 2021 of the electronic databases SCOPUS and Web of Science. These databases were used in this review for presenting several advantages such as quality control of articles, multidisciplinary focus and the number of publishers indexed.

To explore and obtain the largest possible number of studies that investigated well-being and ill-being in the workplace using the SDT theoretical framework, no time frame was defined during the search process.

Finally, the following search terms were used: “‘ill-being’ OR ‘well-being’” AND “self-determination theory” AND “work OR experiment” in titles, keywords and abstracts. Keywords “work” and “experiment” were included to refine the studies conducted in the work environment or experimental studies whose results could be generalized for this context.

Inclusion/exclusion criteria

To be included in the review, all studies were assessed based on the inclusion/exclusion criteria displayed in Table 1. This review has aimed to analyze empirical results arising from working contexts, or that could be generalized to the working population. Therefore, empirical research conducted with samples composed of children, adolescents, elderly, or entirely with people with a previous diagnosis were excluded, since the settings were very specific and distinct from the focus of this review.

Furthermore, the inclusion criterion for validated measures was considered to allow comparability and standardization between variables, mainly due to the proliferation of concepts and measures of well-being and BPN in current scientific research (Martela and Ryan, 2021).

A total of 44 studies were included in this review. The initial search retrieved 655 articles. Exclusion of duplicates (n = 170) reduced this number by 485 and a further 166 studies were excluded if inclusion criteria were not met in title or abstract. A review of the full text resulted in 275 studies being excluded. All references retrieved from the database were added to reference management software (Endnote X9).

All 44 articles included in this review were published in English, between the years of 1993 and 2020.

Finally, despite the claim that ill-being has received far less attention from empirical researchers than well-being (Deci et al., 2017; Van den Broeck et al., 2021), this asymmetry was not observed in this systematic review. In fact, as can be observed in Table 2, ill-being has been explored in a greater number of samples than well-being when investigating the effect of intrinsic motivation (30 samples for ill-being and 24 samples for well-being) and amotivation (09 samples for ill-being and 03 samples for well-being). In addition, when considering the BPN, the asymmetry depends on the psychological mechanism under study: well-being has been explored in association with SBPN in a greater number of samples (a total of 216 for well-being and 90 for ill-being), while ill-being is more studied when associated with FBPN (a total of 43 for ill-being and 25 for well-being).

Data coding and analyses

Studies characteristics and empirical results regarding well-being/ill-being predictors, contextual antecedents and consequences were summarized in tables. Evidence regarding each SDT construct – motivation, SBPN and FBPN – was calculated based on the percentage of independent samples supporting each association with well-being/ill-being indicators, with a significance level set at 0.05 for both bivariate (BA) and multivariate analysis (MA). A system was adopted to classify the associations between SDT constructs and well-being/ill-being (Teixeira et al., 2012): positive (++) or negative (−−) for percentage ≥75% and (+) or (−) for percentage between 50 and 75% showing associations in both BA and MA; 0/+ or 0/− when the evidence was split between no association (0) and positive/negative associations, respectively; and (?) for results indicating inconsistent or indeterminate/unknown results due to the small number of studies available.

Results

A total of 44 articles were included in this review and comprised 53 independent samples (see Appendix 1). The number of samples was higher than the number of studies because seven studies analyzed data from more than one sample or reported results concerning more than one SDT construct (Gillet et al., 2012, 2018; Graves and Luciano, 2013; Huyghebaert et al., 2018; Kibler et al., 2019; Lok and Dunn, 2020; Olafsen and Bentzen, 2020; Olafsen and Frølund, 2018; Osin et al., 2018). The samples' main characteristics are displayed in Figure 2.

Evidence regarding the association between each SDT construct – motivation, SBPN and FBPN – and well-being/ill-being outcomes are summarized in Table 2.

Contribution of motivation to well-being/ill-being

To assess the impact of motivation on workers' psychological health, reviewed studies employed a total of 13 different measures of well-being and 13 different indicators of ill-being. While work engagement is the most used indicator for well-being (used in seven samples), emotional exhaustion is the most employed measure of ill-being (also employed in seven samples). In addition, when addressing well-being experiences, seven samples relied solely on hedonic manifestations (Graves and Luciano, 2013; Kibler et al., 2019; Nie et al., 2015; Olafsen and Bentzen, 2020; Olafsen and Frølund, 2018; Tóth‐Király et al., 2020), five samples relied solely on eudaimonic measures (Chambel et al., 2015; Dagenais-Desmarais et al., 2018; Gillet et al., 2018; Lopes and Chambel, 2017; Lopes et al., 2019) and five samples combined measures of both perspectives (Chambel and Sobral, 2019; Gillet et al., 2018; Olafsen and Bentzen, 2020; Osin et al., 2018). When comparing the effect of motivation on hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives of well-being (Table 3), it is observed that associations follow the same pattern across the different types of motivation, except for introjected (which presented positive association with hedonic indicators and no association with eudaimonic manifestations) and external motivation (which presented a lack of relationship with eudaimonic indications and inconsistent associations with hedonic manifestations).

In addition, self-determined forms of motivation (i.e. intrinsic, integrated, identified and autonomous) demonstrate a consistent pattern of positive association with well-being indicators in the samples considered in the analysis. Regarding ill-being, evidence is not conclusive since results are split between negative and the lack of association. It is noteworthy, however, that integrated motivation was addressed in a small number of samples and the associations with both well-being and ill-being were investigated only in BA.

The evidence on the other side of the motivational spectrum is not that consistent: introjected and external motivation has no significant association with well-being in most samples, while evidence regarding controlled motivation is divided between positive association and non-existent. Concerning ill-being, most samples indicate that both external and controlled motivation either have a positive or no association with the phenomena, while findings regarding introjected motivation are inconclusive.

Finally, only three studies explored the relationship between amotivation and well-being and evidence was split between negative and lack of association. For ill-being, the situation differs greatly: studies suggest a consistent positive relationship between the variables.

Contribution of BPN to well-being/ill-being

To assess the contribution of BPN on workers' psychological health, reviewed studies employed a total of 20 different measures of well-being and 16 different indicators of ill-being. While job (work) satisfaction is the most used indicator for well-being (used in nine samples), emotional exhaustion is the most employed measure of ill-being (employed in six samples). In addition, when specifically addressing well-being phenomena, 14 samples relied solely on hedonic manifestations (Ebersold et al., 2019; Eriksson and Boman, 2018; Giebe and Rigotti, 2020; Graves and Luciano, 2013; Kang and Yoo, 2019; Rayburn, 2014; Rouse et al., 2019, van Hooff and De Pater, 2019), five samples relied on eudaimonic measures (Collie et al., 2015; Domenech-Betoret et al., 2015; Dose et al., 2019; Robijn et al., 2020; Elst et al., 2012) and 11 samples combined measures of both perspectives (Babenko, 2018; Chen et al., 2020; Desrumaux et al., 2015; Gatt and Jiang, 2020; Gillet et al., 2012, 2019; Ilardi et al., 1993; Meng, 2020; Shir et al., 2019; Slemp and Vella-Brodrick, 2013). When comparing the effect of BPN on hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives of well-being, the same pattern of associations was observed when taking into consideration the composite score for SBPN and FBPN (Table 4) and for each basic need (both satisfaction and frustration) (Table 5). However, while competence satisfaction is strongly and positively associated with eudaimonic indications, its association with hedonic manifestations is split between positive and absent.

In nine studies, a composite score was created to represent in a single measure satisfaction/frustration of autonomy, competence and relatedness. In general, unanimous evidence shows that SBPN is positively associated with well-being indicators and negatively associated with ill-being.

FBPN follows a similar but inverse pattern, despite the smaller number of studies that used the measure: there is a negative relationship with well-being and a positive relationship with ill-being. However, this last evidence is not unanimous since one sample presented a negative association with ill-being, measured in the form of emotional exhaustion.

When considering each need separately, the satisfaction of all three basic needs demonstrates a consistent and positive association with well-being indicators in most samples under analysis. Regarding ill-being, evidence consistently indicates a negative association for all three needs considered. It is important to mention that evidence suggesting the absence of association between SBPN and both well-being and ill-being was also present in a small number of samples, specifically in results derived from MA.

Results regarding the frustration of each need have been very consistent in indicating a negative association with well-being and a positive relationship with ill-being, especially when considering the needs for autonomy and relatedness. Evidence regarding the association between competence, frustration and well-being was mostly negative, but the absence of association was also observed. Results for ill-being are more consistent, indicating unanimously a positive association.

Individual/workplace contribution to well-being/ill-being

Besides studying the contribution of SDT variables for the occurrence of well-being/ill-being in working contexts, 34 studies were also dedicated to investigating the influence of individual and workplace variables for the occurrence of the phenomena (see Appendix 2).

Individual variables

For individual measures (Table 6), evidence suggests positive association between well-being and active engagement in entrepreneurship, creative thinking, hardiness, optimism, self-efficacy, self-esteem, tenure in temporary agency work and tolerance for ambiguity. Concerning prosocial motivation, evidence was split between positive and the absence of association between the variables for samples studied (i.e. employees and entrepreneurs) (Kibler et al., 2019).

Concerning ill-being, evidence suggests that neuroticism has a positive association, while emotional stability, met expectations and optimism have a negative association with the phenomenon studied, meaning that individuals with neuroticism traits are more likely to experience ill-being, while those with emotional stability, met expectations and optimism are less susceptible to negative experiences. Moreover, age and gender seem to have no association with ill-being, while evidence regarding hours worked per week and tenure in temporary agency work present inconclusive findings.

Workplace variables

Thirty-three workplace variables and their relationship with well-being and ill-being, were investigated and classified in three categories: (a) organization environment and practices (n = 15); (b) relationship between employee and leader/supervisor (n = 7); and (c) job/task content or execution (n = 11).

For organization environment and practices (Table 7), findings indicate that empowerment, investiture and serial socialization and procedural justice have a positive impact on well-being and a negative association with ill-being. Besides, perception of organizational support – from administration, colleagues, family and friends and psycho-pedagogical – positively impacts well-being, while perceived organizational support from administration, colleagues and family and friends diminishes ill-being. Additionally, psychosocial safety climate, that is, when an organization implements practices to protect workers' psychological health, has a negative association with ill-being.

Non-territorial working (or unassigned desks) and remote work were found to have no association with the variables investigated. For job climate, material and psycho-pedagogical support resources, evidence was inconclusive.

Regarding the relationship between employee and leader/supervisor (Table 8), leader–member exchange, managerial need support and open conflict norms are shown to positively impact well-being, but only leader–member exchange tends to diminish ill-being. Inconclusive evidence was found regarding perceived supervisor autonomy support and perceptions of supervisor controlling behaviors. Finally, while engaging leadership does not seem to promote well-being, manager appreciation and managerial need support did not present significant influence on ill-being.

Among the variables concerning job/task content or execution (Table 9), job crafting (cognitive, relational and task) seems to enhance well-being. However, well-being seems to decrease as job demands and job insecurity increase, while job complexity has no significant association.

Besides, evidence indicates that job insecurity and time pressure is conditions that increase ill-being, while job complexity, again, has no significant association with the phenomenon.

Finally, results regarding job challenges, job hindrances, perceived fit, role ambiguity and time pressure are not conclusive regarding the association with well-being or ill-being.

Consequences of well-being/ill-being

Among six studies investigating the consequences of well-being/ill-being (see Appendix 3), evidence indicates that well-being promotes positive outcomes for organizations and workers: well-being tends to enhance employees' absorptive capacity, affective commitment, goal attainment, job satisfaction, positive attitudes towards digital workplace transformation and opportunity recognition capability (Table 10).

Results also indicate that affective commitment and goal attainment are reduced in the presence of ill-being. On the other hand, ill-being is found to increase the intention to leave profession and school.

Discussion

The aim of this systematic review has been to provide a comprehensive analysis regarding the relationship between SDT and well-being/ill-being in working contexts, based on empirical research on the subject. In this study, two electronic databases, Web of Science and Scopus, were used to identify studies that were published in English and Portuguese up to February 2021, however only publications in English met all the inclusion criteria for this review. In addition, only studies with validated scales were included, for reasons of reliability and comparability between measures, but a wide range of measurement tools were identified in the reviewed studies. Moreover, studies included in this review were characterized by distinct methodological designs, different working contexts and countries.

  1. Motivation. When addressing motivation, there is a preponderance of samples that relied solely on hedonic measures to empirically operationalize well-being phenomena in the workplace. While the hedonic perspective of well-being refers to short term happiness and pleasure, the eudaimonic approach concerns self-realization and personal growth (Ryan and Deci, 2001). In this regard, despite their differences, Huta (2015) argues that both approaches are complementary, since people who experience both hedonia and eudaimonia display higher levels of subjective well-being. Therefore, to comprehensively assess well-being experiences, it is important that studies include measures of both perspectives, which happened in only five samples. This situation may result in a limited comprehension of well-being phenomena in the workplace. Additionally, the reviewed studies also reinforce associations already well-established within SDT: (a) self-determined forms of motivation tend to promote well-being among workers. Moreover, evidence indicates that when workers engage in behaviors for enjoyment, identity, or personal values, negative feelings decrease (Deci et al., 2017; Deci and Ryan, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000b); and (b) extrinsically motivated behaviors, as well as amotivation, have a detrimental effect on well-being, while promoting ill-being among workers (Deci et al., 2017; Deci and Ryan, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000b). However, it is worth mentioning that, among self-determined types of motivation, fewer studies were dedicated to investigating integrated motivation (i.e. pursuing a behavior because it is part of one's identity) (Deci and Ryan, 1985). Indeed, previous studies have already questioned the distinctiveness of this type of regulation in relation to intrinsic and identified motivations, which possibly explains the reason it has not been included in most scales and empirical research (Gagné et al., 2015; Howard et al., 2017; Van den Broeck et al., 2021). In fact, in this review, integrated, intrinsic and identified motivations followed the same pattern of association with well-being. A possible explanation is that individuals themselves could not distinguish the actual reasons driving their behavior, constituting a consistency bias (Van den Broeck et al., 2021), or that existing scales were not capable of capturing the essence of integrated motivation, in opposition to other types of self-determined motivation. In addition, results regarding the relationship between ill-being and integrated regulation are supported by only one sample, which prevents any generalization.

Introjected regulation occurs when people engage in activities for ego-involvement and self-administered rewards or punishments (Deci and Ryan, 1985). Although reviewed evidence concerning the effect of this type of motivation on workers' psychological health is not conclusive, a considerable amount of evidence indicates positive associations with both well-being and ill-being indicators. Similar results were found by Van den Broeck et al. (2021) in a meta-analytical study, suggesting that, in work contexts, contingent self-esteem is less detrimental to psychological health than financial rewards to orient desired behaviors. In addition, when considering hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives of well-being, results indicated that introjected regulation is positively associated with hedonic well-being while it has no association with the eudaimonic approach, suggesting that behaviors driven by self-administered rewards or punishments may lead to short-term pleasant emotions but do not lead to subjective feelings of personal growth or self-realization.

Regarding external motivation, SDT establishes that this form of regulation, based on material rewards, can have a powerful effect in motivating desired behaviors, but it brings long-term decreases in well-being (Deci et al., 2017). In this review, results suggest a lack of relationship between external motivation and well-being, challenging SDT assumptions; however, this relationship was investigated mostly in cross-sectional studies, which do not take into account the long-term effect mentioned by Deci et al. (2017), only one study employed a 6-month prospective design to investigate external regulation and still found no association with psychological health at work over time (Dagenais-Desmarais et al., 2018). Moreover, a similar result regarding external motivation was identified among the elderly population (Tang et al., 2020), with the authors hypothesizing that this situation may be due to the diversity of scales used, specificities of the population studied, or even that external motivation can be associated only with specific indicators of well-being. In this regard, taking into consideration the effect of external motivation on both perspectives of well-being (i.e. hedonic and eudaimonic), results have demonstrated the absence of effect on eudaimonic indicators, indicating that external rewards do not lead to positive feelings of personal growth; on the other hand, evidence regarding the association between this type of regulation and hedonic manifestations is inconclusive, requiring further investigations to explore the potential effects of material rewards on workers' short happiness.

In this review, evidence regarding autonomous and controlled motivation were in accordance with SDT principles, especially regarding the association with well-being: autonomous regulated behaviors are consistently associated with the experience of well-being, while controlled motivation has a negative or no effect on well-being (Deci et al., 2017; Deci and Ryan, 1985). On the other hand, the role of autonomous and controlled motivation in ill-being was the subject of fewer studies. In addition, it is worth mentioning that, for controlled regulation, consistent evidence indicates that behaviors oriented for extrinsic reasons do not have the power to mitigate negative feelings on workers.

Evidence about the effects of amotivation on well-being are inconclusive, possibly due to the small number of samples that investigated this association. However, the lack of motivation is positively associated with ill-being. This result finds support in meta-analytical (Van den Broeck et al., 2021) and empirical studies with other populations (Baker, 2004; Cresswell and Eklund, 2005). Amotivation is a state where the absence of motivation leads to purposeless and non-intentional behaviors, resulting in feelings of incompetence and uncontrollability towards an activity (Baker, 2004; Deci and Ryan, 1985); if self-determined types of regulation are associated with well-being because of SBPN, the relationship between amotivation and ill-being may occur because non-regulated behaviors involve FBPN.

  1. SBPN/FBPN. Similar to motivation, studies that addressed BPN (both SBPN and FBPN) employed preponderantly hedonic measures to empirically operationalize well-being phenomena in the workplace. Again, this limitation may result in a limited comprehension of how the satisfaction or frustration of BPN may impact both perspectives of positive psychological functioning in the workplace. However, the difference of associations between competence satisfaction and hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives of well-being observed in the reviewed samples suggests that the feeling of efficacy promoted among workers is not necessarily associated with short-term pleasure but has impacts on long-term perception of personal growth and self-actualization. In addition, the results condensed in this review are strongly congruent with SDT principles, indicating that BPN is an important mechanism to explain both “the bright and dark side” of psychological functioning in the workplace: while SBPN leads to well-being, FBPN is associated with ill-being (Deci et al., 2017; Longo et al., 2016, 2018). In addition, while Martela and Ryan (2021) argue that, to comprehensively assess well-being in individuals, SBPN indicators are essential measures, this review suggests that the same logic applies to indicators of FBPN and ill-being, at least in working contexts. However, in a meta-analytic study, Van den Broeck et al. (2016) argue that BPN is a more powerful mechanism in predicting well-being than ill-being; this result may be due to the fact that FBPN measures were not included in the meta-analysis, as even the authors suggest. Indeed, the way FBPN is measured in the studies included in this review seems a fundamental aspect in this discussion: SBPN and FBPN are independent but related constructs with different effects on psychological health (Longo et al., 2016, 2018), which means that not being able to do things (i.e. low or no satisfaction of needs) is different from being prevented from doing things (i.e. frustration of needs). Among the four scales used to measure FBPN, W-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010) was developed to assess specifically SBPN, but items were reverse-scored to measure need frustration (Elst et al., 2012); besides, BPNSFS (Chen et al., 2015), used in three studies, included ambiguous items such as “I feel insecure about my abilities” and “I feel disappointed with much of my performance”, which seem conceptually closer to not satisfying competence need than being frustrated by not satisfying such necessity, for example. It is possible that these conceptual fragilities regarding frustration assessment may have led to inaccurate measurements of the constructs, possibly affecting associations with workers' psychological health outcomes. In fact, such methodological fragilities regarding the scales employed to measure FBPN, combined with the scarcity of studies that included measures of FBPN, prevent generalizations concerning the independence between the constructs of SBPN and FBPN in working context, which have already been empirically identified in other contexts of human experience (Bartholomew et al., 2011; Cordeiro et al., 2016; Longo et al., 2016, 2018; Van den Broeck et al., 2016). In addition, studies included in this review relied on samples of workers from different industries, countries and cultures, a fact that may have some influence on the subjective importance of each basic need to attain well-being (Deci et al., 2001).

  2. Individual and workplace contribution. Considering that a central SDT concern relates to the development of organizational environments that promote development of skills, performance and well-being (Ryan and Deci, 2000b), this systematic review also aimed at identifying contextual variables that favor well-being and prevent/mitigate ill-being among workers. Evidence suggests that positive individual resources such as active engagement in entrepreneurship, creative thinking, hardiness, optimism, self-efficacy and self-esteem present higher levels of well-being. In fact, both optimism (i.e. the belief that good things will happen) and self-efficacy (i.e. the ability to deal with unforeseen events) are regarded as important personal resources and have been empirically associated with high levels of work engagement (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017; Bakker and Sanz-Vergel, 2013; Xanthopoulou et al., 2013). Conversely, ill-being is elevated when workers have neuroticism traits and is reduced in the face of emotional stability, met expectations, optimism and support from family and friends. Regarding workplace variables, three distinct categories were identified: (a) organization environment and practices; (b) relationship between employee and leader/supervisor; and (c) job/task content or execution. Evidence indicates that work contexts that enable workers to actively engage in their activities in a fair, open and supportive environment are more favorable to workers' mental health, by increasing well-being and reducing ill-being. On the other hand, job insecurity and time pressure seem to have detrimental effects on workers. In addition, it is noteworthy that, among the 25 studies – and 26 samples – that investigated the effect of contextual variables on workers' well-being and ill-being, 19 studies relied on the mechanisms of BPN to investigate this association. However, only four studies have included measures of FBPN (Ebersold et al., 2019; Giebe and Rigotti, 2020; Huyghebaert et al., 2018; Elst et al., 2012). The limited number of studies dedicated to investigate the effect of contextual variables on FBPN supports the claim that unsuccessful motivational processes have received less attention from SDT researchers (Deci et al., 2017), which, consequently, prevents the elucidation of the contextual causes that lead to the occurrence of the phenomenon of interest. Moreover, despite the simultaneous and independent nature of the subjective mechanisms of SBPN and FBPN (Bartholomew et al., 2011; Cordeiro et al., 2016; Diener and Emmons, 1984; Longo et al., 2016, 2018), among the studies that investigated the effect of contextual variables in the occurrence of FBPN, only two studies included measures of both SBPN and FBPN (Ebersold et al., 2019; Giebe and Rigotti, 2020). As a result, while there is a consistent body of empirical literature that have contributed to the elucidation of the environmental conditions and organizational practices associated with the psychological mechanism of SBPN, the same cannot be said about the FBPN phenomenon, which has been addressed in far fewer empirical studies, as demonstrated in the review. Additionally, while SDT assumptions focus on psychological mechanisms such as SBPN/FBPN and motivation, some of the studies reviewed have benefited from the integration of SDT assumptions with other theoretical frameworks such as the Personality Potential Model (Osin et al., 2018), Response Styles Theory (Kranabetter and Niessen, 2019) and Job Demand–Control Support Model (Chambel et al., 2015; Chambel and Sobral, 2019; Perry et al., 2018) to investigate how individual and workplace characteristics promote well-being/ill-being in employees. Most studies included in this review, however, relied on the combination of SDT with Job Demands-Resources Model (JD-R Model) (Chambel et al., 2015; Chambel and Sobral, 2019; Desrumaux et al., 2015; Domenech-Betoret et al., 2015; Dose et al., 2019; Giebe and Rigotti, 2020; Gillet et al., 2018; Huyghebaert et al., 2018; Olafsen and Frølund, 2018; Robijn et al., 2020; Rubino et al., 2009). JD-R Model distinguishes two features associated with work environments and psychological outcomes: job demands are associated with ill-being as they require employees' continuous psychological and/or physical effort, while job resources facilitate the accomplishment of tasks and lead to work engagement and other positive outcomes (Bakker and Demerouti, 2007; Schaufeli and Bakker, 2004). In general, reviewed studies provide evidence that SBPN/FBPN mechanisms mediate the link between job characteristics (i.e. job demands and resources) and employees' experience of well-being/ill-being and other work outcomes, indicating that the integration of SDT and JD-R Model is a powerful and much valued theoretical tool to comprehensively assess well-being/ill-being in working contexts.

  3. Consequences. It is well established within SDT and overall managerial literature that when workers have a positive feeling towards their job, positive individual and organizational outcomes will emerge; the inverse assumption is also vastly supported – negative psychological functioning can have detrimental effects on both workers' and organizations (Huyghebaert et al., 2018; Longo et al., 2016, 2018). Despite the importance of the subject, few studies have investigated the consequences of well-being and ill-being, moreover no study in this review explored potential effects that well-being can have on negative consequences. Well-being and ill-being are considered independent constructs that can occur simultaneously in individuals (Bartholomew et al., 2011; Bradburn and Caplovitz, 1965; Cordeiro et al., 2016; Diener and Emmons, 1984; Longo et al., 2016, 2018) and empirical results suggest that ill-being can both increase negative consequences (Ford et al., 2019) and reduce positive work outcomes (Gatt and Jiang, 2020; Olafsen, 2017). For well-being, however, studies were limited to investigating its effects on positive work outcomes (Chen et al., 2020; Collie et al., 2015; Gatt and Jiang, 2020; Meske and Junglas, 2020; Olafsen, 2017). Potential mitigating effects of well-being on negative work outcomes have not been explored in empirical research and constitute an important topic for both management academics and practitioners.

Limitations and directions for future research

This systematic review presents several limitations: (1) it included studies published only in English (no study published in Portuguese fulfilled the inclusion criteria) and important findings in other languages may have been left out of this review; and (2) in-progress or unpublished studies with potential significant results were not included in this review.

Review studies also presented some deficiencies that can be addressed in future research: (1) study design: experimental and prospective studies are fewer than cross-sectional and although they require more time and other resources, they can better explain the causal relationship between variables, specifically regarding the relationship between external motivation and well-being; (2) measurement tools: reviewed studies employed a great variety of scales to measure the variables of interest (e.g. BPN, motivation, well-being and ill-being), a fact that can lead to inconsistent findings between studies focusing on the same variables. Besides, all instruments are self-reporting, which can lead to biased measurements of constructs; (3) scarcity of research on FBPN: in this review, despite the claim that fewer studies have address ill-being phenomena in the workplace (Deci et al., 2017; Van den Broeck et al., 2021), the asymmetry was not observed in the samples studied. However, most studies relied solely on needs satisfaction measures to investigate the experience of ill-being. Given that recent empirical evidence suggests that this phenomenon is best predicted by basic needs frustration, this inaccurate approach can impair the proper understanding of ill-being in current working contexts; (4) selection of proper scales to measure FBPN: in this review, numerous scales were used to measure FBPN, however it is important to select measures that properly capture the construct conceptual essence (i.e. being prevented to satisfying BPN) to avoid inaccurate measurements of the construct; (5) scarcity of studies that comprehensively assess well-being phenomena: in this review, it was observed a preponderance of hedonic measures of well-being, to the detriment of eudaimonic indicators or the combination of both approaches, which would provide a more comprehensive assessment of well-being in the workplace; (6) scarcity of studies on the effect of well-being/ill-being on work outcomes: although reviewed results reinforce that well-being is associated with positive work outcomes, while ill-being is associated with negative results, few empirical studies investigated these relationships, which are of great relevance for both management academics and practitioners. In addition, it is necessary to clarify the potential effects that well-being can have on negative consequences; (7) difficulty in delimiting ill-being indicators: the lack of a consistent definition of what constitutes the experience of ill-being in the workplace (beyond the opposition to the concept of well-being) may have led to difficulties in defining the indicators that were used in the reviewed studies to measure workers' negative subjective experiences; (8) scarcity of studies on new forms and contexts of work: in recent years, a combination of factors such as technology, economic crisis and more recently the Covid-19 pandemic, has allowed traditional forms of work to be replaced by new arrangements, in which workers have more flexibility and autonomy over their work; as work changes, resources and demands associated tend to change too, impacting workers' mental health and work outcomes. In this new scenario, an important change concerns the way individuals are being compensated for their work; the effects of distinct compensation systems on behavior regulation and SBPN/FBPN and consequently on workers' mental health, remain an underexplored but relevant topic for future research.

Theoretical and practical contributions

Regarding theoretical contributions, empirical evidence gathered in this systematic review supports SDT assumptions concerning the role of basic needs and motivation in the occurrence of both ill-being and well-being in a work context. Furthermore, this study revealed that: (1) integrated motivation does not seem to be empirically distinct from intrinsic and identified regulation in promoting well-being; (2) introjected behaviors may be less harmful to psychological health than externally oriented ones; (3) the relationship between external motivation and well-being/ill-being requires further prospective investigations to be adequately understood; and (4) amotivation seems to have a strong detrimental effect on workers' psychological health, possibly due to FBPN. Finally, this review contributes to the advancement of scientific research by presenting an unprecedented framework that aggregates empirical findings regarding the antecedents, predictors and consequences of ill-being and well-being in working contexts, which enables the identification of research gaps and deficiencies that can be addressed in future studies on the subject.

In addition, this systematic review adds to a growing body of empirical evidence that demonstrates that ill-being experiences such as burnout and stress do not simply stem from SBPN, but rather result directly from FBPN. This implies that, to prevent the occurrence of ill-being among employees, it is important that managers implement organizational practices and policies designed not only to promote SBPN, but also with the purpose to prevent or mitigate FBPN, given the independence between these phenomena.

In this regard, this systematic review provides information for human resources management practitioners to design work environments and practices that promote employees' psychological health. Given that the basic needs of autonomy, relatedness and competence are satisfied and/or frustrated by favorable and unfavorable environmental conditions, respectively (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Deci and Ryan, 2008b; Ryan, 2009; Ryan and Deci, 2019; Van den Broeck et al., 2010, 2016), managerial implications can be suggested based on results obtained in this study in order to: (1) facilitate SBPN among workers and (2) prevent FBPN among workers.

Given that SBPN has been vastly addressed in SDT empirical research, this systematic review provides organizations and managers with a wide array of organizational policies and practices that have been empirically associated both to SBPN and well-being experiences among workers: autonomy over office spaces, autonomy support (from supervisor and organization), empowerment, positive job climate, job crafting, job complexity, leader-member exchange, manager appreciation, open conflict norms, socialization (investiture and serial) and support resources (from colleagues, administration, material and psycho-pedagogical).

On the other hand, in order to prevent the occurrence of FBPN – and consequently ill-being – among workers, reviewed studies have empirically demonstrated that increased autonomy support and psychosocial safety climate prevent workers' perception of FBPN, while time pressure tends to increase the occurrence of such phenomenon among workers. In addition, it is important that organizations constantly evaluate the effectiveness of their practices and policies intended to promote SBPN to identify and change potential work conditions that prevent employees' satisfaction of needs and therefore, leads to FPN and ill-being among employees.

Conclusions

In conclusion, evidence gathered in this systematic review consistently confirms SDT tenets regarding the role of self-determined motivation and SBPN in promoting well-being and positive work outcomes, while diminishing ill-being among workers. Empirical evidence reviewed also reinforces recent empirical findings regarding the role of FBPN in the occurrence of ill-being, while evidencing the need for proper measurement for the construct to be adequately investigated in future empirical studies. In fact, methodological fragilities regarding measurement tools and the scarcity of research on FBPN prevent further conclusions regarding the independence between the constructs of SBPN and FBPN.

Moreover, this review goes beyond in providing an unprecedented comprehensive view of the role of contextual variables in favoring the experience of well-being/ill-being, as well as their potential consequences for the worker and organization, indicating that a fair, open and supportive environment tends to be more favorable to workers' mental health, increasing well-being and decreasing ill-being. Nevertheless, while ill-being can both lead to negative consequences and diminish positive outcomes, potential effects of well-being on negative consequences have not been addressed in the studies reviewed. Moreover, results reinforce the claim that SDT empirical research has focused mainly on positive aspects of human functioning, with far fewer studies investigating unsuccessful motivational processes (Deci et al., 2017; Van den Broeck et al., 2021).

Overall, SDT aims to contribute to the development of work environments that enable the development of workers' skills, performance and well-being (Ryan and Deci, 2000b). Therefore, the main contributions of this review are: (1) identifying and differentiating well-being and ill-being predictors in work environments; (2) indicating contextual and individual conditions that empirically are shown to promote employees' mental health; and (3) verifying the effects arising from well-being and ill-being on work outcomes. Based on this information, it is possible to identify perspectives for future research in SDT as well as useful guidelines for practitioners to design healthy work environments.

Figures

PRISMA flow diagram of study selection process

Figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram of study selection process

Summary of samples characteristics

Figure 2

Summary of samples characteristics

Review inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion criteriaExclusion criteria
1. Empirical studies based on STD framework1. Empirical studies that report research with children, adolescents, elderly people, or an entire sample with a prior diagnosis of any specific physical or mental condition
2. Empirical studies set in working context, or experimental studies – to generalize results to working population
3. Empirical studies that employed a validated measure for SBPN/FBPN, motivation, well-being and ill-being
4. Empirical studies that report research on adults, over the age of 18 years2. Theoretical or review studies
5. Papers available in English and Portuguese up to February 2021

Summary of associations between SDT predictors and well-being/ill-being outcomes

MotivationWell-beingIll-being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Intrinsic6(18)72.3(100)0(0)27.7(0)+13(17)7.5(6)38.5(70.5)54(23.5)0/–
Integrated0(4)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Identified5(15)100(100)0(0)0(0)++8(12)12.5(8.5)25(75)75(16.5)0/–
Introjected2(13)50(46)0(0)50(54)03(12)33(41.5)0(25)67(33.5)?
External2(13)0(31)50(7.5)50(61.5)03(12)33(58)0(0)67(42)0/+
Autonomous14(12)79(100)7(0)14(0)++4(7)25(43)25(57)50(0)0/–
Controlled11(11)0(27)27.5(55)63.5(18)0/–4(7)25(71)0(0)75(29)0/+
Amotivation1(2)0(0)100(0)0(100)?2(7)100(57)0(28.5)0(14.5)+
SBPNN+0 N+0 = 
Autonomy29(35)72.5(91.5)0(0)27.5(8.5)+11(19)0(5.3)72.6(89.4)27.4(5.3)
Competence29(34)65.5(91)3.5(0)31(9)+11(18)0(5.5)81.8(77.8)18.2(16.7)– –
Relatedness30(36)66.7(89)0(0)33.3(11)+9(19)0(5.3)77.8(89.4)22.2(5.3)– –
Composite score10(13)100(100)0(0)0(0)++1(2)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
FBPNN+0 = N+0 = 
Autonomy3(5)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –5(7)100(100)0(0)0(0)++
Competence3(5)0(0)67.7(75)33(35)5(7)100(100)0(0)0(0)++
Relatedness3(4)0(0)100(75)0(25)– –5(6)100(100)0(0)0(0)++
Composite score0(2)0(0)0(100)0(0)?3(5)100(80)0(20)0(0)++

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Summary of associations between motivation with hedonic/eudaimonic perspectives of well-being outcomes

Well-being indicatorsIntrinsicIntegratedIdentifiedAutonomous
N+0 = N+0N+0 = N+0 = 
Eudaimonia2 (9)50 (100)0 (0)50 (0)+0 (3)0 (100)0 (0)0 (0)?2 (9)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++7 (9)86 (100)0 (0)14 (0)++
Hedonia4 (9)83.5 (100)0 (0)16.5 (0)++0 (1)0 (100)0 (0)0 (0)?3 (6)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++7 (3)72 (100)14 (0)14 (0)+
 = 6 (18)72.3 (100)0 (0)27.7 (0)+0 (4)0 (100)0 (0)0 (0)?5 (15)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++14 (12)79 (100)7 (0)14 (0)++
Well-Being indicatorsIntrojectedExternal ControlledAmotivation
N+0 = N+0 = N+0 = N+0 = 
Eudaimonia1 (9)44.5 (0)0 (0)100 (55.5)01 (9)0 (33.5)0 (11)100 (55.5)07 (9)0 (33.5)28.5 (44.5)71.5 (22)–/00 (0)0 (0)0 (0)0 (0)?
Hedonia1 (4)100 (50)0 (0)0 (50)+1 (4)0 (25)100 (0)0 (75)–/04 (2)0 (0)50 (0)50 (0)–/01 (2)0 (0)100 (0)0 (100)–/0
 = 2 (13)50 (46)0 (0)50 (54)02 (13)0 (31)50 (7,5)50 (61,5)011 (11)0 (27)27.5 (55)63.5 (18)0/1 (2)0 (0)100 (0)0 (100)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results

Summary of associations between composite overall BPN satisfaction/frustration with hedonic/eudaimonic perspectives of well-being outcomes

N+0 = 
Overall BPN satisfaction
Hedonia5 (9)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++
Eudaimonia4 (2)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++
Combined1 (2)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++
 = 10 (13)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++
Overall BPN Frustration
Hedonia0 (2)0 (0)0 (100)0 (0)?
 = 0 (2)0 (0)0 (100)0 (0)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results

Summary of associations between composite BPN satisfaction/frustration of each need with hedonic/eudaimonic perspectives of well-being outcomes

Well-being indicatorsAutonomyCompetenceRelatedness
N+0 = N+0 = N+0 = 
BPN satisfaction
Eudaimonia12 (12)75 (75)0 (0)25 (25)++12 (12)83.5 (100)0 (0)16.5 (0)++14 (14)78.5 (86)(0)21.5 (14)++
Hedonia14 (19)79 (100)0 (0)21 (0)++14 (18)43 (83.5)7 (0)50 (21.5)+/013 (18)54 (89)0 (0)46 (11)++
Combined3 (4)67 (100)0 (0)33 (0)++3 (4)100 (100)0 (0)0 (0)++3 (4)67 (100)0 (0)33 (0)++
 = 29 (35)72.5 (91.5)0 (0)27.5 (8.5)+29 (34)65.5 (91)3.5 (0)31 (9)+30 (36)66.7 (89)0 (0)33.3 (11)+
BPN Frustration
Hedonia2 (4)0 (0)100 (100)0 (0)– –2 (4)0 (0)50 (50)50 (50)0/1 (3)0 (0)100 (33.5)0 (66.5)0/
Eudaimonia1 (1)0 (0)100 (100)0 (0)– –1 (1)0 (0)100 (100)0 (0)– –1 (1)0 (0)100 (100)0 (0)– –
 = 3 (5)0 (0)100 (100)0 (0)– –3 (5)0 (0)67.7 (75)33 (35)3 (4)0 (0)100 (75)0 (25)– –

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results

Summary of associations between individual variables and well-being/ill-being outcomes

Individual variablesWell-beingIll-being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Active engagement in entrepreneurship1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Age0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(33)0(67)?
Creative thinking0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Emotional stability0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Gender0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(33)0(0)0(67)?
Hardiness1(0)100(0)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Hours worked per week0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(33)0(33)0(33)?
Met expectations0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Neuroticism0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++
Optimism2(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Prosocial motivation2(2)0(100)0(0)100(0)0/+2(2)50(0)0(50)50(50)0
Self-efficacy1(0)100(0)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Self-esteem1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Tenure in temporary agency work0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(0)0(100)?
Tolerance for ambiguity1(0)100(0)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Summary of associations between variables regarding organization environment and practices and well-being/ill-being outcomes

Organization environment and practicesWell-beingIll-being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Empowerment0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Investiture socialization0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Job climate1(1)0(100)0(0)100(0)0/+1(1)0(0)0(100)100(0)0/–
Non-territorial working0(1)0(0)0(0)0(100)?0(1)0(0)0(0)0(100)?
Organizational support4(5)75(100)0(0)25(0)++2(2)0(0)67(100)33(0)– –
Organizational autonomy support3(5)100(100)0(0)0(0)++3(5)0(0)67(60)33(40)
Procedural justice0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Psychosocial safety climate0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Remote work0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(67)0(0)100(33)?
Serial socialization0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Support resources: administration0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Support resources: colleagues0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Support resources: family and friends0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Support resources: material0(1)0(50)0(50)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(50)0(50)?
Support resources: psycho-pedagogical0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(50)0(50)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Summary of associations between variables regarding the relationship between employee and leader/supervisor and well-being/ill-being outcomes

Relationship between employee and leader/supervisorWell-beingIll-being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Engaging leadership0(1)0(0)0(0)0(100)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Leader–member exchange3(3)100(67)0(0)0(33)+1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –
Manager appreciation0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(0)0(100)100(0)0/–
Managerial overall need support0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(0)0(100)?
Open conflict norms0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Perceived autonomy support from supervisor1(2)0(100)0(0)100(0)0/+0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Perceptions of supervisor controlling behaviors1(1)0(0)0(100)100(0)0/–0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Summary of associations between variables regarding job/task content or execution and well-being/ill-being outcomes

Job/task content or executionWell-beingIll-being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Cognitive crafting1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Job challenges: workload and cognitive demands1(1)0(100)0(0)100(0)0/+0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Job complexity1(1)0(0)0(0)100(100)01(1)0(0)0(0)100(100)0
Job demands1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)− −1(1)0(100)100(0)0(0)?
Job hindrances: work-home interference and worry1(1)0(0)0(100)100(0)0/–0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Job insecurity1(1)0(0)100(100)0(0)– –1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++
Perceived fit0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)0(0)33(67)67(33)?
Relational crafting1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Role ambiguity0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(1)33(67)0(0)67(33)?
Task crafting1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Time pressure1(1)0(0)100(0)0(100)0/–1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Summary of associations between well-being/ill-being and variables of organizational/individual consequences

ConsequencesWell-BeingIll-Being
N+0 = N+0 = 
Absorptive capacity1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Affective commitment0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Attitudes towards digital workplace transformation1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Goal attainment0(1)0(100)0(0)0(0)?0(1)0(0)0(100)0(0)?
Intent to leave profession0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(0)100(0)0(0)0(0)?
Intent to leave school0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?1(0)100(0)0(0)0(0)?
Job satisfaction1(1)50(100)0(0)50(0)+0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Opportunity recognition capability1(1)100(100)0(0)0(0)++0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?
Organizational commitment1(1)0(100)0(0)100(0)0/+0(0)0(0)0(0)0(0)?

Note(s): Results derived from multivariate analyses and bivariate analyses (in parenthesis). N, number of samples; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (0), association not significant; (?), inconsistent findings or indeterminate results or unknown

Description of reviewed studies (SDT predictors)

AuthorsDesignSamplePredictorsIndicatorsAnalysis
SizeContextCountryMeasureWell-beingIll-beingWell-beingIll-being
Motivation
Chambel et al. (2015)Cross-sectional1,045SoldiersPortugalMotivation at Work Scale (MAWS) (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Autonomous (+)
Controlled (−)
MA: Autonomous (+)
Controlled (−)
BA: Autonomous (−/−)
Controlled (+/+)
MA: Autonomous (−)
Controlled (+)
Work engagementEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBivariate correlations (BC); Structural Equation Modeling (SEM)
Chambel and Sobral (2019)Cross-sectional3,300Temporary agency workersPortugalMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Intrinsic (+/+/+/+/+)
Integrated (+/+/+/+/+)
Identified (+/+/+/+/+)
Introjected (+/+/+/ns/+)
External (+/+/+/ns/ns)
BA: Intrinsic (−/−)
Integrated (−/−)
Identified (−/−)
Introjected (−/−)
External (ns/ns)
Vigor, dedication and absorption; General wellbeing; Health perceptionsEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC
Dagenais-Desmarais et al. (2018)Prospective805General workersCanadaMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Intrinsic (+)
Identified (+)
Introjected (+)
External (+)
MA: Intrinsic (ns)
Identified (+)
Introjected (ns)
External (ns)
BA: Intrinsic (−)
Identified (−)
Introjected (−)
External (ns)
MA: Intrinsic (ns)
Identified (−)
Introjected (ns)
External (ns)
Psychological well-being at workBurnoutBC; SEM
Gillet et al. (2018) – study 1Cross-sectional328General workersFranceMultidimensional Work Motivation Scale (MWMS) (Gagné et al., 2015)BA: Intrinsic (+/+)
Identified (+/+)
Introjected (ns/ns)
External (-/ns)
Work engagement; Quality of working lifeBC
Gillet et al. (2018) – study 2Cross-sectional521General workersFranceMWMS (Gagné et al., 2015)BA: Intrinsic (+/+/+)
Identified (+/+/+)
Introjected (ns/ns/ns)
External (ns/ns/ns)
BA: Intrinsic (−/−)
Identified (−/−)
Introjected (+/+)
External (ns/ns)
Work engagement; Quality of working life; Work satisfactionGlobal job-anxiety; BurnoutBC
Graves and Luciano (2013)Cross-sectional283General workersUSAMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Identified (+/+)
Intrinsic (+/+)
MA: Autonomous (+/+)
Job satisfaction; Subjective vitalityBC; SEM
Kibler et al. (2019)Prospective186EntrepreneursUKIntrinsic motivation with Situational Motivation Scale (Guay et al., 2000)BA: Intrinsic (+)
MA: Intrinsic (ns)
BA: Intrinsic (−)
MA: Intrinsic (ns)
Life satisfactionStressBC; Path model analysis
Kibler et al. (2019)Prospective544EmployeesUKIntrinsic motivation with Situational Motivation Scale (Guay et al., 2000)BA: Intrinsic (+)
MA: Intrinsic (+)
BA: Intrinsic (−)
MA: Intrinsic (−)
Life satisfactionStressBC; Path model analysis
Lopes and Chambel (2017)Prospective196Temporary agency workersPortugalMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Autonomous (+/+/+)
Controlled (ns/-/ns)
MA: Autonomous (+)
Controlled (ns)
BA: Autonomous (−/−)
Controlled (+/ns)
MA: Autonomous (ns)
Controlled (ns)
Vigor, dedication and absorptionEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC; SEM
Lopes et al. (2019)Cross-sectional3,983Temporary agency workersPortugalMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Autonomous (+/+/+)
Controlled (+/+/+)
MA: Autonomous (+)
Controlled (ns)
BA: Autonomous (−/−)
Controlled (ns/−)
MA: Autonomous (−)
Controlled (ns)
Vigor, dedication and absorptionEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC; SEM
Nie et al. (2015)Cross-sectional266TeachersChinaWork tasks motivation scale for teachers -adapted (Fernet et al., 2008)BA: Intrinsic (+)
Identified (+)
Introjected +)
External (−)
Amotivation (−)
MA: Intrinsic (+)
Identified (+)
Introjected (+)
External (−)
Amotivation (ns)
BA: Intrinsic (−/−)
Identified (−/−)
Introjected (ns/ns)
External (+/+)
Amotivation (+/+)
MA: Intrinsic (ns/–)
Identified (ns/ns)
Introjected (ns/+)
External (+/ns)
Amotivation (+/+)
Job satisfactionWork stress; Illness symptomsBC; Path analysis
Olafsen and Frolund (2018)Cross-sectional160EntrepreneursMAWS (Gagné et al., 2010)BA: Autonomous (+)
MA: Autonomous (+)
Subjective vitalityBC; SEM
Olafsen and Bentzen (2020) – study 1Cross-sectional239Organization employeesNorwayMWMS (Gagné et al., 2015)MA: Identified (+/+)
Intrinsic (+/+)
MA: Identified (ns/ns/–)
Intrinsic (ns/ns/–)
Positive affect; Life satisfactionNegative affect; Somatic symptom burden; Emotional exhaustionLatent profile analysis (LPA)
Olafsen and Bentzen (2020) – study 2Cross-sectional207Organization employeesNorwayMWMS (Gagné et al., 2015)MA: Identified (+/−)
Intrinsic (+/−)
MA: Identified (−/+)
Intrinsic (−/+)
Vigor; Sleep qualityEmotional exhaustion; Work–home interferenceLPA
Osin et al. (2018) – study 1Cross-sectional4,708Production employeesRussiaProfessional Motivation Questionnaire (Osin et al., 2013)BA: Autonomous (+/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/−/–)
Controlled
(−/−/−/−/−/−/−/−/+/+)
MA: Autonomous
(+/+/ns/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/+)
Controlled (ns/−/−/+/ns/+/+/+/ns/ns/+)
Life Satisfaction; Work engagement; Job Satisfaction (salary, work conditions, management, colleagues and job process); Organization commitment; Work/life imbalance and life/work imbalanceBC; Moderated mediation models
Osin et al. (2018) – study 2Prospective372Production employeesRussiaProfessional Motivation Questionnaire (Osin et al., 2013)MA: Autonomous (ns/+/ns/+) (process of work and work conditions)
Controlled (ns/ns/ns/ns/ns)
Life satisfaction; Work engagement; Organizational commitment; Job satisfactionMultiple regression models
Rubino et al. (2009)Cross-sectional284Self-employed individualsIntrinsic motivation scale (Pelletier et al., 1995)BA: Intrinsic (ns/ns/−)
MA: Intrinsic (ns/ns/–)
Emotional exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacyBC; Hierarchical regression analyses
Tóth‐Király et al. (2020)Cross-sectional955General workersHungaryMWMS (Gagné et al., 2015)BA: Intrinsic (+)
Identified (+)
Introjected (ns)
External – material (ns)
External – social (ns)
Amotivation (−)
BA: Intrinsic
(–/ns/+/ns/–)
Identified (−/+/ns/–/ns)
Introjected (+/+/ns/ns/+)
External (material) (+/ns/ns/ns/+)
External (social) (+/+/+/+/+)
Amotivation
(+/−/–/ns/+)
Work satisfactionGlobal, Physical, Cognitive and Emotional exhaustion; Work addictionBC
Basic psychological needs
Babenko (2018)Cross-sectional57PhysiciansCanadaBasic Psychological Needs at Work (BPNWS) (Brien et al., 2012)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+/ns)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
MA: Autonomy (ns/ns)
Competence (ns/ns)
Relatedness (+/+)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
MA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
Professional life satisfaction; Work-related engagementExhaustion (emotional, physical and cognitive)BC; Multivariate regression analysis
Chen et al. (2020)Cross-sectional234Creative entrepreneursTaiwanWork-related Basic Need Satisfaction (W-BNS) (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (+)
MA: Relatedness (+)
Physical, psychological and social well-beingBC; SEM
Collie et al. (2015)Cross-sectional485TeachersCanadaW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness w/colleagues (+/+)
Relatedness w/students (+/+)
MA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/ns)
Relatedness w/colleagues (+/+)
Relatedness w/students (+/ns)
General well-being; Teacher well-beingBC; SEM
Desrumaux et al. (2015)Cross-sectional298TeachersFranceBPNWS (Brien et al., 2012) Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
MA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
Psychological well-beingDistressBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Domenech-Betoret et al. (2015)Cross-sectional282TeachersSpainTeacher psychological needs (Doménech-Betoret, 2013)Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (+/+)
Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
MA: Overall (+)
Satisfaction
Relatedness (−/−)
Autonomy (−/−)
Competence (−/−)
MA: Overall (−)
Vigor and dedicationEmotional exhaustion and depersonalizationBC; SEM
Dose et al. (2019)Cross-sectional224Professional counselorsFranceBasic Psychological Needs in Sport– adapted (Gillet et al., 2008)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Psychological well-being at workBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Ebersold et al. (2019)Cross-sectional49TeachersGermanyBasic Psychological Need Satisfaction and Frustration (BPNSFS) (Chen et al., 2015)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+/+)
Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (ns/+)
Satisfaction
BA: Overall (−/−)
Autonomy (−/−)
Competence (ns/ns)
Relatedness (−/−)
Life satisfaction; Positive affectEmotional exhaustion; Negative affectBC
Frustration
BA: Overall (−/−)
Autonomy (−/−)
Competence (–/ns)
Relatedness (−/−)
Frustration
BA: Overall (−/+)
Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
Eriksson and Boman (2018)Cross-sectional1,200General workersSwedenBasic Psychological Need Satisfaction at Work (BPNS-W) (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Psychological well-beingBC; Hierarchical regression analysis
Ford et al. (2019)Cross-sectional1,556TeachersUSABPNSFS (Chen et al., 2015)Satisfaction
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
BurnoutMultilevel path analysis
Gatt and Jiang (2020)Cross-sectional139Organization employeesNew ZealandRelatedness with W-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (+/+)
Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (−)
Work engagement; Job satisfactionEmotional exhaustionBC
Giebe and Rigotti (2020)Prospective308General workersGermanyBPNSFS (Chen et al., 2015)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
MA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (ns)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
MA: Autonomy (ns)
Competence (−)
Job satisfactionEmotional exhaustionBC; Multilevel path analysis
Frustration
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (ns)
Frustration
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (ns)
Gillet et al. (2012) – study 1Cross-sectional468General workersFranceBasic Psychological Needs in Sport (Gillet et al., 2008)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+/+/+)
MA: Overall (+/+/+)
Work satisfaction; Happiness; Self-realizationBC; SEM
Gillet et al. (2012) – study 2Cross-sectional650General workersFrancePsychological Need Thwarting (PNTS) (Bartholomew et al., 2011)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+/+/+)
MA: Overall (+/+/+)
Work satisfaction; Happiness; Self-realizationBC; SEM
Frustration
BA: Overall (−/−/–)
MA: Overall (−/−/–)
Gillet et al. (2019)Prospective294NursesFranceBasic Psychological Needs in Sport– adapted (Gillet et al., 2008)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+/+/+/+) Competence (+/+/+/+)
Relatedness (+/+/+/+)
MA: Autonomy (+/ns/ns/+)
Competence (+/+/+/+)
Relatedness (ns/ns/ns/ns)
Vigor, dedication and absorption; Job satisfactionBC; SEM
Graves and Luciano (2013)Cross-sectional283General workersUSABPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
MA: Autonomy (ns/ns)
Competence (ns/+)
Relatedness (ns/ns)
Job satisfaction; Subjective vitalityBC; SEM
Hewett et al. (2017)Cross-sectional416General workersUK, Belgium, Denmark and GermanyW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010) at work and homeSatisfaction at work
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/ns)
Relatedness (ns/ns)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (ns)
Relatedness (ns)
Satisfaction at work
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (−/−)
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (ns)
Relatedness (ns)
Positive affectNegative affectBC; Multilevel models analysis
Satisfaction at home
BA: Autonomy (ns/+)
Competence (ns/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Satisfaction at home
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (ns/−)
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
Huyghebaert et al. (2018) – study 1Cross-sectional269NursesFrancePsychological Need Thwarting at Work (EFBPT) (Gillet et al., 2012)Frustration
BA: Overall (+/+)
MA: Overall (+/+)
Work-family conflict; Turnover intentionsBC; SEM
Huyghebaert et al. (2018) – study 2Prospective393NursesFranceEFBPT (Gillet et al., 2012)Frustration
BA: Overall (+)
MA: Overall (+)
BurnoutBC; SEM
Ilardi et al. (1993)Cross-sectional117Factory workersUSAWork Motivation Form-Employee (Kasser et al., 1992)Satisfaction
MA: Autonomy (+/+/ns)
Competence (ns/ns/+)
Relatedness (ns/+/ns)
Satisfaction
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (ns)
Relatedness (ns)
Satisfaction with the work performed and job satisfaction; Self-esteemPresence of nonpsychotic psychiatric disordersRegression
analyses
Kang and Yoo (2019)Cross-sectional218Music TeachersUSAMusic Teachers' Psychological Need Measure, modified from Psychological Need Measure (Johnston and Finney, 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Overall well-beingBC; Multiple regression analysis
Kranabetter and Niessen (2019)Prospective194Energy branch employeesGermanyBPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−/−)
Competence (−/−)
Relatedness (−/−)
Ruminative thoughts; Depressive symptomsBC
Lok and Dunn (2020) – study 3Experimental100Amazon MTurk workersSocial connection scale-revised (Lee et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (+)
Satisfaction
BA: Relatedness (−)
Positive affectNegative affectANOVA
Lok and Dunn (2020) – study 4Experimental100Amazon MTurk workersDaily Autonomy scale (Reis et al., 2000)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−)
Positive affectNegative affectANOVA
Meng (2020)Cross-sectional275TeachersChinaW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
MA: Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/−)
Relatedness (ns/+)
Self-efficacy; Job satisfactionBC; SEM
Meske and Junglas (2020)Cross-sectional149Organization employeesGermanyBPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Well-beingBC
Olafsen (2017)Prospective115Health professionalsNorwayW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
Subjective well-beingBurnoutBC
Olafsen and Frolund (2018)Cross-sectional160EntrepreneursW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (ns)
Subjective vitalityBC; SEM
Perry et al. (2018) – study 2Cross-sectional145General employeesUSABPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−/−/−)
Competence (−/−/−)
Relatedness (−/−/−)
MA: Autonomy (−/−/−)
Competence (−/−/−)
Relatedness (−/−/−)
Exhaustion and disengagement; Job dissatisfactionBC; SEM
Rayburn (2014)Cross-sectional226Service workersUSABPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (ns)
Relatedness (+)
Positive affectBC; SEM
Robijn et al. (2020)Prospective133Public insurance company employeesBelgiumW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+)
MA: Overall (+)
Work engagementBC; SEM
Rouse et al. (2019)Cross-sectional2,236FirefightersUKBPNSFS (Chen et al., 2015)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (ns)
Relatedness (+)
Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (−/−/–)
Competence (−/−/–)
Relatedness (−/−/–)
MA: Autonomy (−/−/–)
Competence (ns/ns/–)
Relatedness (ns/–/ns)
Life satisfactionStress; Depression; AnxietyBC; Hierarchical regression analyses
Frustration
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (ns)
Frustration
BA: Autonomy (+/+/+)
Competence (+/+/+)
Relatedness (+/+/+)
MA: Autonomy (+/+/+)
Competence (+/+/+)
Relatedness (+/+/+)
Shir et al. (2019)Cross-sectional251EntrepreneursSwedenBPNS-W (Deci et al., 2001)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+/+/+/+)
Autonomy (+/+/+/+)
Competence (+/+/+/+)
Relatedness (+/+/+/+)
Well-being indexBC
Slemp and Vella-Brodrick (2013)Cross-sectional334General workersAustraliaIntrinsic Need Satisfaction (Baard et al., 2004)Satisfaction
BA: Overall (+/+)
Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
MA: Overall (+/+)
Autonomy (+/+)
Competence (+/+)
Relatedness (+/+)
Positive emotions; Positive psychological functioningBC; SEM
van Hooff and De Pater (2019)Cross-sectional109Full-time working internsW-BNS (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Satisfaction
BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
Positive energyBC; multi-level analyses
Elst et al. (2012)Cross-sectional3,185General employeesBelgiumW-BNS reverse (Van den Broeck et al., 2010)Frustration
BA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
MA: Autonomy (−)
Competence (−)
Relatedness (−)
Frustration
BA: BA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
MA: Autonomy (+)
Competence (+)
Relatedness (+)
VigorEmotional exhaustionBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)

Note(s): BA, bivariate analysis; MA, multivariate analysis; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (ns), not significant

Description of reviewed studies (context variables)

AuthorsDesignSampleContext variablesPredictorsMeasuresAnalysis
SizeContextCountryWell-beingIll-beingWell-beingIll-being
Individual
Chambel and Sobral (2019)Cross-sectional1,045SoldiersPortugalTenure in temporary agency workBA: +BA: ns/nsWork engagementEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC
Chen et al. (2020)Cross-sectional234Creative entrepreneursTaiwanCreative thinkingBA: +Physical, psychological and social well-beingBC
Desrumaux et al. (2015)Cross-sectional298TeachersFranceOptimismBA: + MA: +BA: − MA: −Psychological well-beingDistressBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Domenech-Betoret et al. (2015)Cross-sectional282TeachersSpainSupport resources: family and friendsBA: +/+BA: −/−Vigor and dedicationEmotional exhaustion and depersonalizationBC
Dose et al. (2019)Cross-sectional224Professional counselorsFranceSelf-esteemBA: + MA: +Psychological well-being at workBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Kibler et al. (2019)Prospective186/544Entrepreneurs and EmployeesUKProsocial motivationEntrepreneurs
BA: + MA: ns
Entrepreneurs
BA: ns MA: +
Life satisfactionStressBC; Path analysis
Employees
BA: + MA: ns
Employees
BA: ns MA: −
Osin et al. (2018)Cross-sectional4,708Production employeesRussiaOptimismMA: +/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/−/−Life satisfaction; Work engagement; Job satisfaction (salary, work conditions, management, colleagues and job process); Organization commitment; Work/life imbalance and life/work imbalanceMultiple regression analyses
Self-efficacyMA: +/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/−/−
Tolerance for ambiguityMA: +/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/ns/ns
HardinessMA: +/+/+/+/+/+/+/+/−/−
Perry et al. (2018)Cross-sectional145General employeesUSAEmotional stabilityBA: −/−/−
MA: −/−/−
Exhaustion and disengagement; Job dissatisfactionBC; Hierarchical ordinary least squares regression
Rubino et al. (2009)Cross-sectional284Self-employed individualsAgeBA: –/ns/nsEmotional exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacyBC; Hierarchical regression analysis
GenderBA: +/ns/ns
NeuroticismBA: +/+/+
MA: +/+/+
Met expectationsBA: –/–/–
MA: –/–/–
Hours worked per weekBA: +/ns/–
Shir et al. (2019)Cross-sectional251EntrepreneursSwedenActive engagement in entrepreneurshipBA: + MA: +Well-being indexBC; SEM
Workplace
Chambel et al. (2015)Cross-sectional3,300Temporary agency workersPortugalOrganizational supportBA: + MA: +BA: −/− MA: −Work engagementEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC; SEM
Leader–member exchangeBA: + MA: +BA: −/− MA: −
Collie et al. (2015)Cross-sectional485TeachersCanadaPerceived autonomy supportBA: +/+General well-being; Teacher well-beingBC
Desrumaux et al. (2015)Cross-sectional298TeachersFranceJob demandsBA: − MA: −BA: +
MA: −
Psychological well-beingDistressBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Organizational resources (Job climate)BA: + MA: nsBA: − MA: ns
Domenech-Betoret et al. (2015)Cross-sectional282TeachersSpainSupport resources: colleaguesBA: +/+BA: −/−Vigor and dedicationEmotional exhaustion and depersonalizationBC
Support resources: administrationBA: +/+BA: −/−
Support resources: materialBA: ns/+BA: −/ns
Support resources: psycho-pedagogicalBA: +/+BA: −/ns
Dose et al. (2019)Cross-sectional224Professional counselorsFranceLeader–member exchangeBA: + MA: +Psychological well-being at workBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)
Ebersold et al. (2019)Cross-sectional49TeachersGermanPerceived autonomy supportBA: +/+BA: −/−Life satisfaction; Positive affectEmotional exhaustion; Negative affectBC
Gatt and Jiang (2020)Cross-sectional139Organization employeesNew ZealandNon-territorial workingBA: ns/nsBA: nsWork engagement; Job satisfactionEmotional exhaustionBC; SEM
Autonomy over office spacesBA: +/+ MA: ns/+BA: − MA: −
Giebe and Rigotti (2020)Prospective308General workersGermanyJob complexityBA: ns MA: nsBA: ns MA: nsJob satisfactionEmotional exhaustionBC; Multilevel path analysis
Time pressureBA: ns MA: –BA: + MA: +
Gillet et al. (2012a)Cross-sectional468General workersFrancePerceived organizational supportBA: +/+/+
MA: +/ns/+
Work satisfaction; Happiness; Self-realizationBC; SEM
Perceived supervisor autonomy supportBA: +/+/+
MA: ns/ns/ns
Perceptions of supervisor controlling behaviorsBA: −/−/−
MA: ns/ns/ns
Gillet et al. (2018)Cross-sectional328General workersFrancePerceived organizational supportBA: +/+/+BA: −/−Work engagement; Quality of working life; Work satisfactionGlobal job-anxiety; BurnoutBC
Procedural justiceBA: +/+/+BA: −/−
Gillet et al., 2019Prospective294NursesFranceSupervisors' autonomy-supportBA: +/+/+/+Vigor, dedication and absorption; Job satisfactionBC
Graves and Luciano (2013)Cross-sectional283General workersUSALeader-Member exchangeBA: +/+ MA: +/nsJob satisfaction; Subjective vitalityBC
Huyghebaert et al. (2019)Prospective393NursesFrancePsychosocial safety climateBA: −/− MA: −/−Work-family conflict; Turnover intentionsBC; SEM
Kranabetter and Niessen (2019)Prospective194Energy branch employeesGermanyManager appreciationBA: −/− MA: ns/nsRuminative thoughts; Depressive symptomsBC
Ordinary least squares regression analysis
Kibler et al. (2019)Prospective186EntrepreneursUKAutonomy at workBA: + MA: +BA: ns MA: nsLife satisfactionStressBC; Path analysis
Kibler et al. (2019)Prospective544EmployeesUKAutonomy at workBA: + MA: +BA: ns MA: nsLife satisfactionStressBC; Path analysis
Lopes et al. (2019)Cross-sectional3,983Temporary agency workersPortugalOrganizational support (agency)BA: +/+/+ MA: +BA: −/− MA: nsVigor, dedication and absorptionEmotional exhaustion and cynicismBC; SEM
Organizational support (client)BA: +/+/+
MA: +
BA: −/−
MA: −
Nie et al. (2015)Cross-sectional266TeachersChinaPerceived autonomy supportBA: + MA: +BA: −/− MA: −/−Job satisfactionWork stress; Illness symptomsBC; Path analysis
Olafsen (2017)Longitudinal115Health professionalsNorwayManagerial need supportBA: +BA: −Subjective well-beingBurnoutBC
Olafsen and Frolund (2018)Cross-sectional160EntrepreneursJob challenges (workload and cognitive demands)BA: + MA: nsSubjective vitalityBC; Path analyses
Job hindrances (work-home interference and worry)BA: − MA: ns
Perry et al. (2018)Cross-sectional145General employeesUSARemote workBA: ns/−/−
MA: ns/ns/ns
Exhaustion and disengagement; Job dissatisfactionBC; SEM
Perceived autonomyBA: −/−/−
MA: −/−/−
Rayburn (2014)Cross-sectional226Service workersUSAEmpowermentBA: +Positive affect; Work engagementBC
Serial socializationBA: +
Investiture socializationBA: +
Robijn et al. (2020)Prospective133Public insurance employeesBelgiumEngaging leadershipBA: nsBC
Open conflict normsBA: +
Rubino et al. (2009)Cross-sectional284Self-employed individualsPerceived fitBA: ns/−/−
MA: ns/ns/−
Emotional exhaustion, cynicism and inefficacyBC; Mediation path analysis
Role ambiguityBA: ns/+/+
MA: ns/ns/+
Slemp and Vella-Brodrick (2013)Cross-sectional334General workersAustraliaTask craftingBA: +/+ MA: +/+Positive emotions; Positive psychological functioningBC; SEM
Relational craftingBA: +/+ MA: +/+
Cognitive craftingBA: +/+ MA: +/+
Elst et al. (2012)Cross-sectional3,185General employeesBelgiumJob insecurityBA: − MA: –BA: + MA: +VigorEmotional exhaustionBC; Hayes and Preacher, 2014 macro for SPSS (Hayes and Preacher, 2014)

Note(s): BA, bivariate analysis; MA, multivariate analysis; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (ns), not significant

Description of reviewed studies (consequence variables)

AuthorsDesignSampleConsequence variablesPredictorsMeasuresAnalysis
SizeContextCountryWell-beingIll-beingWell-beingIll-being
Chen et al. (2020)Cross-sectional234Creative entrepreneursTaiwanOpportunity recognition capabilityBA: + MA: +Physical, psychological and social well-beingBC; SEM
Absorptive capacityBA: + MA: +
Collie et al. (2015)Cross-sectional485TeachersCanadaJob satisfactionBA: +/+ MA: ns/+General well-being; Teacher well-beingBC; SEM
Organizational commitmentBA: +/+ MA: ns/ns
Ford et al. (2019)Cross-sectional1,556TeachersUSAIntent to leave professionMA: +BurnoutMultilevel path analysis
Intent to leave schoolMA: +
Gatt and Jiang (2020) 139Organization employeesNew ZealandAffective commitmentBA: +/+BA: –Work engagement; Job satisfactionEmotional exhaustionBC
Meske and Junglas (2020)Cross-sectional149Organization employeesGermanyAttitudes towards digital workplace transformationBA: + MA: +Well-beingBC; SEM
Olafsen (2017)Prospective115Health professionalsNorwayGoal attainmentBA: +BA: –Subjective well-beingBurnoutBC

Note(s): BA, bivariate analysis; MA, multivariate analysis; (+), positive association; (−), negative association; (ns), not significant

Appendix 1

Table A1

Appendix 2

Table A2

Appendix 3

Table A3

References

Adie, J.W., Duda, J.L. and Ntoumanis, N. (2012), “Perceived coach-autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the well- and ill-being of elite youth soccer players: a longitudinal investigation”, Psychology of Sport and Exercise, Vol. 13 No. 1, pp. 51-59.

Arya, S., Kaji, A.H. and Boermeester, M.A. (2021), “PRISMA reporting guidelines for meta-analyses and systematic reviews”, JAMA Surgery, Vol. 156 No. 8, pp. 789-790.

Baard, P.P., Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2004), “Intrinsic need satisfaction: a motivational basis of performance and weil-being in two work settings”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 34 No. 10, pp. 2045-2068.

* Babenko, O. (2018), “Professional well-being of practicing physicians: the roles of autonomy, competence and relatedness”, Healthcare (Basel), Vol. 6 No. 1, p. 12.

Baker, S.R. (2004), “Intrinsic, extrinsic and amotivational orientations: their role in university adjustment, stress, well-being and subsequent academic performance”, Current Psychology, Vol. 23 No. 3, pp. 189-202.

Bakker, A.B. and Demerouti, E. (2007), “The Job Demands‐Resources model: state of the art”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 309-328.

Bakker, A.B. and Demerouti, E. (2017), “Job demands-resources theory: taking stock and looking forward”, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 273-285.

Bakker, A.B. and Sanz-Vergel, A.I. (2013), “Weekly work engagement and flourishing: the role of hindrance and challenge job demands”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 83 No. 3, pp. 397-409.

Bartholomew, K.J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R.M., Bosch, J.A. and Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011), “Self-determination theory and diminished functioning: the role of interpersonal control and psychological need thwarting”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 37 No. 11, pp. 1459-1473.

Bradburn, N.M. and Caplovitz, D. (1965), Reports on Happiness, Aldine, Chicago.

Brien, M., Forest, J., Mageau, G.A., Boudrias, J.S., Desrumaux, P., Brunet, L. and Morin, E.M. (2012), “The basic psychological needs at work scale: measurement invariance between Canada and France”, Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 167-187.

* Chambel, M.J., Castanheira, F., Oliveira-Cruz, F. and Lopes, S. (2015), “Work context support and Portuguese soldiers' well-being: the mediating role of autonomous motivation”, Military Psychology, Vol. 27 No. 5, pp. 297-310.

* Chambel, M.J. and Sobral, F. (2019), “When temporary agency work is not so temporary”, Economic and Industrial Democracy, Vol. 40 No. 2, pp. 238-256.

* Chen, M.H., Tseng, M and Teng, M.J. (2020), “Creative entrepreneurs' well-being, opportunity recognition and absorptive capacity: self-determination theory perspective”, Entrepreneurship Research Journal, Vol. 10 No. 1, p. 20180171.

Chen, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Beyers, W., Boone, L., Deci, E.L., Van der Kaap-Deeder, L., Duriez, B., Lens, W., Matos, L., Mouratidis, A., Ryan, M.R., Sheldon, K.M., Soenens, B., Van Petegem, S. and Verstuyf, J. (2015), “Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration and need strength across four cultures”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 39 No. 2, pp. 216-236.

* Collie, R.J., Shapka, J.D., Perry, N.E. and Martin, A.J. (2015), “Teachers' psychological functioning in the workplace: exploring the roles of contextual beliefs, need satisfaction and personal characteristics”, Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 108 No. 6, pp. 788-799.

Cordeiro, P., Paixão, P., Lens, W., Lacante, M. and Luyckx, K. (2016), “The Portuguese validation of the basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration scale: concurrent and longitudinal relations to well-being and ill-being”, Psychologica Belgica, Vol. 56 No. 3, pp. 193-209.

Cresswell, S.L. and Eklund, R.C. (2005), “Motivation and burnout in professional rugby players”, Research Quarterly for Exercise Sport, Vol. 76 No. 3, pp. 370-376.

Deci, E.L., Olafsen, A.H. and Ryan, R.M. (2017), “Self-determination theory in work organizations: the state of a science”, Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 19-43.

Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (1985), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior, Plenum, New York and London.

Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2000), “The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior”, Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 227-268.

Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2008a), “Hedonia, eudaimonia and well-being: an introduction”, Journal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 1-11.

Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2008b), “Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development and health”, Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 182-185.

* Dagenais-Desmarais, V., Leclerc, J.S. and Londei-Shortall, J. (2018), “The relationship between employee motivation and psychological health at work: a chicken-and-egg situation?”, Work and Stress, Vol. 32 No. 2, pp. 147-167.

Deci, E.L., Ryan, R.M., Gagné, M., Leone, D.R., Usunov, J. and Kornazheva, B.P. (2001), “Need satisfaction, motivation and well-being in the work organizations of a former eastern bloc country: a cross-cultural study of self-determination”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 27 No. 8, pp. 930-942.

* Desrumaux, P., Lapointe, D., Sima, M.N., Boudrias, J.S., Savoie, A. and Brunet, L. (2015), “The impact of job demands, climate and optimism on well-being and distress at work: what are the mediating effects of basic psychological need satisfaction?”, European Review of Applied Psychology, Vol. 65 No. 4, pp. 179-188.

Diener, E. (1994), “Assessing subjective well-being: progress and opportunities”, Social Indicators Research, Vol. 31 No. 2, pp. 103-157.

Diener, E. (2009), “Subjective well-being”, in Diener, E. (Ed.), The Science of Well-Being: The Collected Works of Ed Diener, Springer Netherlands, Dordrecht, pp. 11-58.

Diener, E. and Emmons, R.A. (1984), “The independence of positive and negative affect”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 47 No. 5, pp. 1105-1117.

Diener, E., Eunkook, E.M., Richard, R.E. and Smith, H.L. (1999), “Subjective well-being: three decades of progress”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 125 No. 2, pp. 276-302.

Diener, E., Oishi, S. and Tay, L. (2018), “Advances in subjective well-being research”, Nature Human Behaviour, Vol. 2 No. 4, pp. 253-260.

Diener, E. and Ryan, K. (2009), “Subjective well-being: a general overview”, South African Journal of Psychology, Vol. 39 No. 4, pp. 391-406.

Doménech-Betoret, F. (2013), “Teacher psychological needs, locus of control and engagement”, Spanish Journal of Psychology, Vol. 16, p. E29.

Domenech-Betoret, F., Lloret-Segura, S. and Gómez-Artiga, A. (2015), “Teacher support resources, need satisfaction and well-being”, Spanish Journal of Psychology, Vol. 18, p. E6.

* Dose, E., Desrumaux, P., Bernaud, J.L. and Hellemans, C. (2019), “What makes happy counselors? From self-esteem and leader-member exchange to well-being at work: the mediating role of need satisfaction”, Europe's Journal of Psychology, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 823-842.

* Ebersold, S., Rahm, T. and Heise, E. (2019), “Autonomy support and well-being in teachers: differential mediations through basic psychological need satisfaction and frustration”, Social Psychology of Education, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 921-942.

* Vander Elst, T., Van den Broeck, A., De Witte, H. and De Cuyper, N. (2012), “The mediating role of frustration of psychological needs in the relationship between job insecurity and work-related well-being”, Work and Stress, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 252-271.

* Eriksson, M. and Boman, E. (2018), “Short is beautiful: dimensionality and measurement invariance in two length of the basic psychological need satisfaction at work scale”, Front Psychology, Vol. 9, p. 965.

Fernet, C., Austin, S. and Vallerand, R.J. (2012a), “The effects of work motivation on employee exhaustion and commitment: an extension of the JD-R model”, Work and Stress, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 213-229.

Fernet, C., Guay, F., Senécal, C. and Austin, S. (2012b), “Predicting intraindividual changes in teacher burnout: the role of perceived school environment and motivational factors”, Teaching and Teacher Education, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 514-525.

Fernet, C., Gagné, M. and Austin, S. (2010), “When does quality of relationships with coworkers predict burnout over time? The moderating role of work motivation”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 31 No. 8, pp. 1163-1180.

Fernet, C., Senécal, C., Guay, F., Marsh, H. and Marsh, M. (2008), “The work tasks motivation scale for teachers (WTMST)”, Journal of Career Assessment, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 256-279.

Fisher, C.D. (2010), “Happiness at work”, International Journal of Management Reviews, Vol. 12 No. 4, pp. 384-412.

Gagné, M. and Deci, E.L. (2014), “The history of self-determination theory in psychology and management”, in Gagné, M. (Ed.), He Oxford Handbook of Work Engagement, Motivation and Self-Determination Theory, Oxford Universiry Press, Oxford.

* Ford, T.G., Olsen, J., Khojasteh, J., Ware, J. and Urick, A. (2019), “The effects of leader support for teacher psychological needs on teacher burnout, commitment and intent to leave”, Journal of Educational Administration, Vol. 57 No. 6, pp. 615-634.

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Gilbert, M., Aubé, C., Morin, E. and Malorni, A. (2010), “The motivation at work scale: validation evidence in two languages”, Educational and Psychological Measurement, Vol. 70 No. 4, pp. 628-646.

Gagné, M., Forest, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Crevier-Braud, L., Van den Broeck, A., Aspeli, A.K., Bellerose, J., Benabou, C., Chemolli, E., Güntert, S.T., Halvari, H., Indiyastuti, D.L., Johnson, P.A., Molstad, M.H., Naudin, M., Ndao, A., Olafsen, A.H., Roussel, P., Wang, Z. and Westbye, C. (2015), “The Multidimensional Work Motivation Scale: validation evidence in seven languages and nine countries”, European Journal of Work Organizational Psychology, Vol. 24 No. 2, pp. 178-196.

* Gatt, G. and Jiang, L. (2020), “Can different types of non-territorial working satisfy employees' needs for autonomy and belongingness? Insights from self-determination theory”, Environment and Behavior, Vol. 53 No. 9, pp. 953-986.

* Giebe, C. and Rigotti, T. (2020), “Tenets of self-determination theory as a mechanism behind challenge demands: a within-person study”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 37 No. 5, pp. 480-497.

*Gillet, N., Fouquereau, E., Forest, J., Brunault, P. and Colombat, P. (2012), “The impact of organizational factors on psychological needs and their relations with well-being”, Journal of Business and Psychology, Vol. 27 No. 4, pp. 437-450.

* Gillet, N., Fouquereau, E., Vallerand, R.J., Abraham, J. and Colombat, P. (2018), “The role of workers' motivational profiles in affective and organizational factors”, Journal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 1151-1174.

* Gillet, N., Le Gouge, A., Pierre, R., Bongro, J., Méplaux, V., Brunault, P., Guyetant, S., Fremont, C., Camus, V., Colombat, P., Fouquereau, E. and Cheyroux, P. (2019), “Managerial style and well-being among psychiatric nurses: a prospective study”, Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, Vol. 26 Nos 7-8, pp. 265-273.

Gillet, N., Rosnet, E. and Vallerand, R.J. (2008), “Développement d’une échelle de satisfaction des besoins fondamentaux en contexte sportif”, Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, Vol. 40 No. 4, pp. 230-237.

* Graves, L.M. and Luciano, M.M. (2013), “Self-determination at work: understanding the role of leader-member exchange”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 37 No. 3, pp. 518-536.

Guay, F., Vallerand, R.J. and Blanchard, C. (2000), “On the assessment of situational intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: the situational motivation scale (SIMS)”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 175-213.

Hayes, A.F. and Preacher, K. (2014), “Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable”, British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, Vol. 67 No. 3, pp. 451-470.

Headey, B., Holmström, E. and Wearing, A. (1984), “Well-being and ill-being: different dimensions?”, Social Indicators Research, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 115-139.

Headey, B., Holmstrom, E. and Wearing, A. (1985), “Models of well-being and ill-being”, Social Indicators Research, Vol. 17 No. 3, pp. 211-234.

Hewett, R., Haun, V.C., Demerouti, E., Rodríguez Sánchez, A.M., Skakon, J. and De Gieter, S. (2017), “Compensating need satisfaction across life boundaries: a daily diary study”, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 90 No. 2, pp. 270-279.

Howard, J.L., Gagné, M. and Bureau, J.S. (2017), “Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: a meta-analysis”, Psychol Bull, Vol. 143 No. 12, pp. 1346-1377.

Huta, V. (2015), “An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts”, in Reinecke, L. and Oliver, M.B. (Eds), The Routledge Handbook of Media Use, Routledge, New York, pp. 32-51.

Huta, V. (2017), “An overview of hedonic and eudaimonic well-being concepts”, Handbook of Media Use and Wellbeing: International Perspectives on Theory and Research on Positive Media Effect, Routldge, London, pp. 14-33.

Huta, V. and Hawley, L. (2008), “Psychological strengths and cognitive vulnerabilities: are they two ends of the same continuum or do they have independent relationships with well-being and ill-being?”, Journal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 11 No. 1, pp. 71-93.

* Huyghebaert, T., Gillet, N., Fernet, C., Lahiani, F.J. and Fouquereau, E. (2018), “Leveraging psychosocial safety climate to prevent ill-being: the mediating role of psychological need thwarting”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 107, pp. 111-125.

* Ilardi, B.C., Leone, D., Kasser, T. and Ryan, R.M. (1993), “Employee and supervisor ratings of motivation: main effects and discrepancies associated with job satisfaction and adjustment in a factory setting”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 23 No. 21, pp. 1789-1805.

Jayawickreme, E., Forgeard, M.J. and Seligman, M.E. (2012), “The engine of well-being”, Review of General Psychology, Vol. 16 No. 4, pp. 327-342.

Johnston, M.M. and Finney, S.J. (2010), “Measuring basic needs satisfaction: evaluating previous research and conducting new psychometric evaluations of the basic needs satisfaction in general scale”, Contemporary Educational Psychology, Vol. 35 No. 4, pp. 280-296.

* Kang, S. and Yoo, H. (2019), “Music teachers' psychological needs and work engagement as predictors of their well-being”, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, Vol. 221, pp. 58-71.

Kasser, T., Davey, J. and Ryan, R.M. (1992), “Motivation and employee-supervisor discrepancies in a psychiatric vocational rehabilitation setting”, Rehabilitation Psychology, Vol. 37 No. 3, pp. 175-188.

*Kibler, E., Wincent, J., Kautonen, T., Cacciotti, G. and Obschonka, M. (2019), “Can prosocial motivation harm entrepreneurs' subjective well-being?”, Journal of Business Venturing, Vol. 34 No. 4, pp. 608-624.

* Kranabetter, C. and Niessen, C. (2019), “Appreciation and depressive symptoms: the moderating role of need satisfaction”, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, Vol. 24 No. 6, pp. 629-640.

La Guardia, J.G., Ryan, R.M., Couchman, C.E. and Deci, E.L. (2000), “Within-person variation in security of attachment: a self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment and well-being”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 79 No. 3, pp. 367-384.

Lee, R.M., Draper, M. and Lee, S. (2001), “Social connectedness, dysfunctional interpersonal behaviors, and psychological distress: testing a mediator model”, Journal of counseling psychology, Vol. 48 No. 3, pp. 310-318.

Lent, R.W. (2004), “Toward a unifying theoretical and practical perspective on well-being and psychosocial adjustment”, Journal of Counseling Psychology, Vol. 51 No. 4, pp. 482-509.

* Lok, I. and Dunn, E.W. (2020), “Under what conditions does prosocial spending promote happiness?”, Collabra-Psychology, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 1-14.

Longo, Y., Alcaraz-Ibáñez, M. and Sicilia, A. (2018), “Evidence supporting need satisfaction and frustration as two distinguishable constructs”, Psicothema, Vol. 30 No. 1, pp. 74-81.

Longo, Y., Gunz, A., Curtis, G.J. and Farsides, T. (2016), “Measuring need satisfaction and frustration in educational and work contexts: the need satisfaction and frustration scale (NSFS)”, Journal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 295-317.

Lopes, S. and Chambel, M.J. (2017), “Temporary agency workers' motivations and well-being at work: a two-wave study”, International Journal of Stress Management, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 321-346.

Lonsdale, C., Hodge, K. and Rose, E. (2009), “Athlete burnout in elite sport: a self-determination perspective”, Journal of Sports Sciences, Vol. 27 No. 8, pp. 785-795.

* Lopes, S., Chambel, M.J. and Cesário, F. (2019), “Linking perceptions of organizational support to temporary agency workers' well-being”, International Journal of Organizational Analysis, Vol. 27 No. 5, pp. 1376-1391.

Martela, F. and Ryan, R.M. (2021), “In selecting measures for a comprehensive assessment of well-being, it is essential to include indicators of psychological need satisfaction”, Preventive Medicine Reports, Vol. 23, 101474.

* Meng, Q. (2020), “Chinese university teachers' job and life satisfaction: examining the roles of basic psychological needs satisfaction and self-efficacy”, Journal of General Psychology, Vol. 149 No. 3, pp. 327-348.

* Meske, C. and Junglas, I. (2020), “Investigating the elicitation of employees' support towards digital workplace transformation”, Behaviour and Information Technology, Vol. 40 No. 11, pp. 11201136.

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. and the PRISMA Group (2009), “Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement”, PLoS Med, Vol. 6 No. 7, e1000097.

* Olafsen, A.H. (2017), “The implications of need-satisfying work climates on state mindfulness in a longitudinal analysis of work outcomes”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 41 No. 1, pp. 22-37.

* Olafsen, A.H. and Bentzen, M. (2020), “Benefits of psychological detachment from work: does autonomous work motivation play a role?”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 11, p. 824.

* Olafsen, A.H. and Frølund, C.W. (2018), “Challenge accepted! Distinguishing between challenge- and hindrance demands”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 33 Nos 4/5, pp. 345-357.

* Nie, Y., Chua, B.L., Yeung, A.S., Ryan, R.M. and Chan, W.Y. (2015), “The importance of autonomy support and the mediating role of work motivation for well-being: testing self-determination theory in a Chinese work organisation”, International Journal of Psychology, Vol. 50 No. 4, pp. 245-255.

Olafsen, A.H., Niemiec, C.P., Halvari, H., Deci, E.L. and Williams, G.C. (2016), “On the dark side of work: a longitudinal analysis using self-determination theory”, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 275-285.

Osin, E., Ivanova, T. and Gordeeva, T. (2013), “Autonomous and controlled professional motivation predict subjective well-being in Russian employees”, Organizacionnaâ psihologiâ (Organizational Psychology), Vol. 3 No. 1, pp. 8-29.

* Osin, E., Ivanova, T., Orel, E. and Rasskazova, E. (2018), “Personality resources and work motivation: a beneficial synergy”, Organizational Psychology, Vol. 8 No 2, pp. 27-46.

Pelletier, L.G., Tuson, K.M., Fortier, M.S., Vallerand, R.J., Briére, N.M. and Blais, M.R. (1995), “Toward a new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: the sport motivation scale (SMS)”, Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 35-53.

* Perry, S.J., Rubino, C. and Hunter, E.M. (2018), “Stress in remote work: two studies testing the Demand-Control-Person model”, European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 27 No. 5, pp. 577-593.

Pussegoda, K., Turner, L., Garritty, C., Mayhew, A., Skidmore, B., Stevens, A., Boutron, I., Sarkis-Onofre, R., Bjerre, L.M., Hróbjartsson, A., Altman, D.G. and Moher, D. (2017), “Systematic review adherence to methodological or reporting quality”, Systematic Reviews, Vol. 6 No. 1, p. 131.

* Rayburn, S.W. (2014), “Improving service employee work affect: the transformative potential of work design”, Journal of Services Marketing, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 71-81.

Reis, H.T., Sheldon, K.M., Gable, S.L., Roscoe, J. and Ryan, R.M. (2000), “Daily well-being: the role of autonomy, competence and relatedness”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 419-435.

Richer, S.F., Blanchard, C. and Vallerand, R.J. (2002), “A motivational model of work turnover”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 32 No. 10, pp. 2089-2113.

* Robijn, W., Euwema, M.C., Schaufeli, W.B. and Deprez, J. (2020), “Leaders, teams and work engagement: a basic needs perspective”, Career Development International, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 373-388.

* Rouse, P.C., Turner, P.J., Siddall, A.G., Schmid, J., Standage, M. and Bilzon, J.L. (2019), “The interplay between psychological need satisfaction and psychological need frustration within a work context: a variable and person-oriented approach”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 44 No. 2, pp. 175-189.

* Rubino, C., Luksyte, A., Perry, S.J. and Volpone, S.D. (2009), “How do stressors lead to burnout? The mediating role of motivation”, Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 289-304.

Ryan, R. (2009), “Self determination theory and well being”, Social Psychology, Vol. 84 No. 822, p. 848.

Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2000a), “The darker and brighter sides of human existence: basic psychological needs as a unifying concept”, Psychological Inquiry, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 319-338.

Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2000b), “Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: classic definitions and new directions”, Contemporary Educational Psychology, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 54-67.

Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2001), “On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 52, pp. 141-166.

Ryan, R.M. and Deci, E.L. (2019), “Brick by brick: the origins, development and future of self-determination theory”, Advances in Motivation Science, Elsevier, Vol. 6, pp. 111-156.

Ryff, C.D. and Singer, B. (2006), “What to do about positive and negative items in studies of psychological well-being and ill-being?”, J Psychotherapy Psychosomatics, Vol. 76 No. 1, p. 61.

Schaufeli, W.B. and Bakker, A.B. (2004), “Job demands, job resources and their relationship with burnout and engagement: a multi-sample study”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 25 No. 3, pp. 293-315.

Sheldon, K.M. and Elliot, A.J. (1999), “Goal striving, need satisfaction and longitudinal well-being: the self-concordance model”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 76 No. 3, pp. 482-497.

Sheldon, K.M., Ryan, R.M. and Reis, H.T. (1996), “What makes for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the person”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 22 No. 12, pp. 1270-1279.

* Shir, N., Nikolaev, B.N. and Wincent, J. (2019), “Entrepreneurship and well-being: the role of psychological autonomy, competence and relatedness”, Journal of Business Venturing, Vol. 34 No. 5, p. 105875.

* Slemp, G.R. and Vella-Brodrick, D.A. (2013), “Optimising employee mental health: the relationship between intrinsic need satisfaction, job crafting and employee well-being”, Journal of Happiness Studies, Vol. 15 No. 4, pp. 957-977.

Tang, M., Wang, D. and Guerrien, A. (2020), “A systematic review and meta-analysis on basic psychological need satisfaction, motivation and well-being in later life: contributions of self-determination theory”, PsyCh Journal, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 5-33.

Teixeira, P.J., Carraça, E.V., Markland, D., Silva, M.N. and Ryan, R.M. (2012), “Exercise, physical activity and self-determination theory: a systematic review”, International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, Vol. 9, p. 78.

* Tóth‐Király, I., Morin, A.J., Bőthe, B., Rigó, A. and Orosz, G. (2020), “Toward an improved understanding of work motivation profiles”, Applied Psychology, Vol. 70 No. 3, pp. 986-1017.

Trépanier, S.G., Fernet, C. and Austin, S. (2013), “Workplace bullying and psychological health at work: the mediating role of satisfaction of needs for autonomy, competence and relatedness”, Work and Stress, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 123-140.

Van den Broeck, A., Ferris, D.L., Chang, C.H. and Rosen, C.C. (2016), “A review of self-determination theory’s basic psychological needs at work”, Journal of Management, Vol. 42 No. 5, pp. 1195-1229.

Van den Broeck, A., Howard, J. L., Van Vaerenbergh, Y., Leroy, H. and Gagné, M. (2021), “Beyond intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: a meta-analysis on self-determination theory’s multidimensional conceptualization of work motivation”, Organizational Psychology Review, Vol. 11 No. 3, pp. 240-273.

Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte, H., Soenens, B. and Lens, W. (2010), “Capturing autonomy, competence and relatedness at work: construction and initial validation of the Work-related Basic Need Satisfaction scale”, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 83 No. 4, pp. 981-1002.

* van Hooff, M.L.M. and De Pater, I.E. (2019), “Daily associations between basic psychological need satisfaction and well‐being at work: the moderating role of need strength”, Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, Vol. 92 No. 4, pp. 1027-1035.

Williams, G.C., Halvari, H., Niemiec, C.P., Sørebø, Ø., Olafsen, A.H. and Westbye, C. (2014), “Managerial support for basic psychological needs, somatic symptom burden and work-related correlates: a self-determination theory perspective”, Work and Stress, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 404-419.

Xanthopoulou, D., Bakker, A.B. and Fischbach, A. (2013), “Work engagement among employees facing emotional demands”, Journal of Personnel Psychology, Vol. 12 No. 2, pp. 74-84.

Corresponding author

Paula Martins Nunes can be contacted at: up201800128@edu.fep.up.pt

Related articles