Co-creativity: possibilities for using the arts with people with a dementia

Hannah Zeilig (University of the Arts, London, UK)
Julian West (Royal Academy of Music, London, UK)
Millie van der Byl Williams (University College London Medical School, London, UK)

Quality in Ageing and Older Adults

ISSN: 1471-7794

Article publication date: 7 June 2018

Issue publication date: 15 August 2018

7285

Abstract

Purpose

The purpose of this paper is to explore the concept of co-creativity in relation to artistic practice with people with a dementia. The aim of the discussion is to outline how co-creativity offers fresh approaches for engaging artists and people with dementia, can contribute to less restrictive understandings of “creativity” and above all, expand the understanding of people with a dementia as creative, relational and agential.

Design/methodology/approach

In order to examine current conceptions of co-creativity and to inform the artistic practice, relevant literature was explored and eight expert interviews were conducted. The interviews were thematically analysed and are included here.

Findings

This paper consequently demonstrates that improvisation, structure, leadership and equality are central elements of co-creative processes and outlines how co-creativity can offer fresh insights into the way in which the arts can engage people with a dementia, the relationship between creativity and dementia and the transformative potential of the co-creative arts for those living with a dementia.

Research limitations/implications

The paper discusses some of the difficulties that are inherent a co-creative approach, including power relations and the limitations of inclusivity. Due to ethical restrictions, the paper is limited by not including the perspectives of people living with a dementia.

Practical implications

This paper paves the way for future research into co-creative processes in a variety of different contexts.

Social implications

A more nuanced understanding of co-creativity with people with dementia could challenge the dominant biomedical and social paradigms that associate “dementia” with irretrievable loss and decline by creating opportunities for creative agency.

Originality/value

This exploration of co-creativity with people with dementia is the first of its kind and contributes to the wider understanding of co-creativity and co-creative practice.

Keywords

Citation

Zeilig, H., West, J. and van der Byl Williams, M. (2018), "Co-creativity: possibilities for using the arts with people with a dementia", Quality in Ageing and Older Adults, Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 135-145. https://doi.org/10.1108/QAOA-02-2018-0008

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2018, Hannah Zeilig, Julian West and Millie van der Byl Williams

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

Co-creativity is a new concept in relation to artistic practice with people with a dementia. This paper traces the origins of this emergent approach. The following discussion situates co-creativity against the wider background of the role of the arts in general for people with a dementia and explores dominant concepts of creativity and how these may be understood in relation to dementia. The notion of co-creativity is then introduced as it has developed from these broader contexts. Our conceptualisation of the term has been informed by the extant literature and also a series of interviews with seven artists and one academic who define their practice as co-creative. Finally, we discuss how co-creativity is currently being practised in the “With All” project (a co-creative arts project that is taking place at the Wellcome Collection, as part of the Created Out of Mind residency). The aim of the discussion is to outline how co-creativity offers fresh approaches for engaging artists and people with dementia, can contribute to less restrictive understandings of “creativity” and above all, expand our understanding of people with a dementia as creative, relational and agential.

The arts for people living with a dementia

The role of the arts for people living with a dementia has been of increasing interest for academics, policy makers and artists alike in the last decade (Camic et al., 2017; Kontos et al., 2015; Kontos, 2018; Zeilig et al., 2015). There has been a growing awareness that art and aesthetics have an important role to play in delivering healthcare as well as a reappraisal of the associations between the arts and society in general. Thus, in recent years, a growing body of international evidence has been compiled exploring the potential of the arts to impact positively on the health and well-being of people living with a dementia (Basting, 2009; Fritsch et al., 2006; Ledger and Edwards, 2011). There has also been a burgeoning awareness that arts-based research methods offer alternative insights into the subjective experiences of people living with dementia (as noted by Bellass et al., 2018).

In this contemporary context, arts activities for people with a dementia have flourished, including initiatives as diverse as singing, drama, visual art, photography, clowning and puppetry to name a few. There has also been an essential growth in the role of the arts for those living with advanced dementias (Crutch et al., 2001). Moreover, there has been recognition of the need to privilege the perspectives and voices of people living with a dementia and the role of the arts in facilitating this (Beard, 2012; Bartlett, 2014; Hara, 2011; Hughes, 2014; McFadden et al., 2008; Zeilig et al., 2014). Although the evidence base requires further strengthening and is still largely under-theorised (Gray et al., 2017; Schall et al., 2017), it is widely accepted that both the participative arts and art therapy are important in a variety of ways for ameliorating the lives of people with a dementia (Creative Health, APPG, 2017).

The purpose of the majority of participative arts projects then is predominantly to promote health, wellbeing, cognitive function and communication and therefore they tend to focus on instrumental benefits for people living with a dementia (de Medeiros and Basting, 2013; Mental Health Foundation, 2011; Salisbury et al., 2011; Young et al., 2015). This is connected with the socio-political context of the arts in health that relies on empirically evidenced medical models of health (White, 2014). A “dose of the arts” is commonly given in measured amounts to people, as if the arts are carefully calibrated medications. Although there are some instances in which the aesthetic purposes of the arts are prioritised – in which the aim is to create a high-quality work of art with and for people living with a dementia (Hatton, 2014; Moss and O’Neill, 2013, 2017; de Medeiros and Basting, 2014; Dupuis et al., 2016), this has not been the main focus of most arts projects. Nevertheless, the therapeutic and aesthetic objectives of a project may overlap and complement one another.

The aims and objectives of participative arts projects differ from the work of art therapists who typically work in clinical settings (hospitals or hospices) and are usually health care professionals who aim to ameliorate specific conditions (Castora-Binkley et al., 2010). Thus, therapists (who may specialise in art, music, drama or visual art) tend to focus on the condition or on one particular symptom and how this can be “treated”. For therapists, the arts are employed as tools to achieve measurable ends (Cowl and Gaugler, 2014).

Although distinct in aims and objectives, art therapy and most participative arts projects for people with a dementia have primarily been focused on the beneficial and instrumental role of the arts and their ability to enhance either the health, wellbeing or quality of life of participants. The arts are often treated as functional under the assumption that they have a measurable impact on individual’s lives. The arts have had a primarily “interventionist” role (Bellass et al., 2018) both in terms of the possibility that they can deliver specific quantifiable benefits for individuals and also in relation to the outputs from arts-based research. Whilst this can be a valuable and often effective approach, it is quite different from deploying the arts for their own sake, in order to engage the innate creativity of people with a dementia. The tendency to overlook the purely creative possibilities of the arts for those with a dementia is connected with entrenched ideas about what creativity is, where it is located and how it is manifested.

Creativity and dementia

The idea of “creativity” coincided with the late Romantic period (Pope, 2005, p. 3) and was closely associated with “art” (Williams, 1988) and with the notion of the artistic “genius”. This idea of creativity as linked to the arts and as an individual trait has been surprisingly persistent (Osborne, 2003). Even recent conceptualisations from both psychological and neurological perspectives, tend to link creative processes to specific, original and tangible acts of production that are associated with individual motivations (for instance: Csikszentmihalyi, 1997; Palmiero et al., 2012). These understandings link to the myth of the “creative individual”, the “genius”, and this powerful motif often shapes social understandings of creative activities. Focusing on the characteristics and capacities of an individual defined as particularly “creative”, the narrative understands creativity as something psychologically inherent to a creative individual (Osborne, 2003) and as associated with what has become known as “Big-C” creativity (Beghetto and Kaufman, 2007). This hegemonic narrative not only informs shared ideas about age and creativity (McMullan and Smiles, 2016) but of central relevance here, also influences the ways in which notions of creativity relate (or more pertinently do not relate) to people living with a dementia. It is perhaps unsurprising then, given the intense dominance of this perspective linking creativity, as a cognitive “trait”, with the activities of particularly gifted individuals that it is not commonly associated with those living with a dementia. Indeed, people with a dementia have only recently been recognised as being routinely capable of creative activity, for instance, in terms of a myriad of everyday “little-c” interactions (Bellass et al., 2018).

In the academic literature, the area of dementia and creativity has predominantly been examined neurologically or from the perspective of clinical psychology. These studies have tended to concentrate on the impact of dementias on the brain systems implicated in creativity and on visual art (de Souza et al., 2010; Gretton and ffytche, 2014; Palmiero et al., 2012). There is a relative lack of consideration of the non-verbal and ephemeral arts, including music and dance. A number of studies have involved the use of standardised measures of creativity as one way of assessing aspects of creative ability (de Souza et al., 2010; Rankin et al., 2007), these clearly present knotty problems of measurement. Indeed, the focus on neuro-scientific perspectives for elucidating the mechanisms involved in creativity and its relationship to the dementias can be both limiting and restrictive in several ways. These perspectives necessarily locate creativity as it exists within particular regions of an individual’s brain (such as the frontopolar cortex) and also highlights the ways in which brain pathology affects creativity in people with a dementia. The overall framework then, is predicated on notions of deficit and loss. The extent to which people living with dementias might contribute to a nuanced understanding of our shared creativity is mostly overlooked, as are the social, relational and emotional features of creative activity.

Therefore, we must realign our discussions about creativity, in order to appreciate the innate capacities for creativity that are evident in all people and that may be discerned in mundane, every day and shared activities (Bellass et al., 2018; Osborne, 2003). We must reconceptualise creativity as collective rather than inherent to any individual and also as associated as much with process as product (Plucker and Beghetto, 2004). Therefore, creativity is not solely located within an individual’s brain but also conditioned by their relationships and wider context. This point is even more pertinent for thinking about creativity with people with a dementia. Moreover, this perspective may extend our understandings of creativity in general by disassociating it from extraordinary individuals or acts and recognising it as a more “banal process of the everyday” (Selberg, 2015, p. 157). In their recent discussion that seeks to broaden the debate on everyday creativity and dementia, Bellass et al. note that:

[…].. using the lens of dementia, it is possible to gain insights into how normative creativity is constructed and accounted for […].

(2018, p. 22)

Co-creativity can add to this debate by offering a new approach for artistic practice with people with a dementia and a means of reconsidering prevailing mores about creativity.

Co-creativity

The term “co-creativity” is nascent. Co-creativity is not defined in the OED and its borders and features remain somewhat indistinct. Therefore, there is currently no unified agreement concerning what this term refers to or how it differs from “collaboration”, “participation” or “co-design” for instance. Nonetheless, it is steadily gaining in popularity, indeed the related term “co-creation” can be found throughout contemporary UK media. It has only recently come into use to describe a particular approach to participatory arts practice (Matarasso, 2017).

Despite the sense that co-creativity is of increasing relevance to both artists and cultural institutions alike, it seems that the approach has not been widely explored, either practically or conceptually. Of particular relevance here, it is almost completely absent from discussions about artistic practice with people living with dementia. A recent search of academic databases (ERIC, CINAHL, MEDLINE, PsycArticles, PsycINFO) revealed less than a dozen relevant papers and only three investigating issues directly related to dementia. The importance of clarifying the meaning of co-creativity is not merely a question of semantics. Once there is a lucid definition of the term we can begin to formulate its conceptual underpinnings.

To date, the more prevalent use of the term “co-creativity” has been in the business or design worlds. The emphasis in these contexts is upon the transfer of value from an end (or predefined) product to a shared process in which all those involved play an integral role in creation (Branco et al., 2017; Prahalad and Ramaswamy, 2004). In these contexts, there is an emphasis on the equal contribution of all involved. However, there is also a focus on selection and filtering in which decisions are made about the most promising contributions.

The possibility that co-creation can erase distinctions between the “producer” and the “consumer” is similarly discussed in the context of arts-based projects (Matarasso, 2017). This leads to an innately democratic and non-hierarchical notion of creativity, in which the diverse capacities of all those involved are woven into the creative process. As Matarasso (2017) further highlights there is no one author, the artists involved do not instruct but rather disperse the knowledge associated with their skills, thereby privileging the creative process. Moreover, there is an important recognition within co-creative practices that creative engagement is an interactive and relational activity that creates community (HE, 2014).

Thus, although there is not currently a single agreed definition for “co-creativity” it is characterised by a number of key features including centrally: a focus on shared process, the absence of a single author or outcome (and instead the idea of shared ownership), inclusivity, reciprocity and relationality. Co-creativity relies on dialogic and empathic approaches (Sennett, 2012) in which, through the process of exchange and discussion, understandings are expanded, although discussions are not necessarily resolved. This is in contrast to dialectic encounters which tend to lead to closure (Sennett, 2012, p. 24). Co-creativity necessitates and creates openness, receptivity and imaginative space. Above all, it contrasts with restrictive notions of the lone creative “genius” that have tended to dominate views of creativity (as mentioned above). On the contrary, here creativity is understood as something that exists within and is shared by the group.

Co-creativity in practice

In order to understand the concept of co-creativity in more depth and in particular the underlying methods that guide its practice; discussions were held with seven artists and one academic who defined their practice as co-creative. A range of informants were deliberately sought who could offer various artistic perspectives, including a musician, a dancer, a writer and poet, a visual artist and several theatre directors; the academic worked using film co-creatively with people with a dementia. These artists were contacted via existing networks (Music for Life, Dementia Positive, the Dementia and Imagination project and Dementia Services Development Centre at Stirling University). All of those who were interviewed work in community or in care home settings with diverse groups of people, including those living with a dementia. The interviews were all transcribed, coded and analysed using a process of emergent thematic analysis that was facilitated by qualitative software (Atlas.ti). This form of thematic analysis is based on content analysis, but is more concerned with patterns rather than frequency. In contrast to classic content analysis, this form of thematic analysis, as used by Dodds et al. (2008), draws upon empirically emergent, rather than theoretically generated themes (Searing and Zeilig, 2017). This was not about quantity, as it is a small-scale qualitative enquiry, but about the richness of the data gathered. Thus, the interviews were read by all authors and in an iterative process a number of recurring themes were identified, these were: improvisation, structure and leadership and equality. Although to some extent they overlap with each other, they are discussed separately below. These themes constitute some of the central mechanisms underlying co-creative practice in the arts and have informed the development of the “With All” project.

Improvisation can be broadly understood as a means of using bodies, space, imagination, objects and instruments in response to the immediate stimuli of one’s environment, without preconceptions (Frost and Yarrow, 2016, p. xv). The central role of improvisation as freeing and as allowing in the moment creation, was clear for all the artists in their co-creative practice. As outlined by one theatre director:

Improvisation is crucial in terms of a way of enabling ideas to surface and a way of seeing just what can happen in the moment.

(Gavin Critchon, Active Enquiry)

Indeed, the importance of emphasising in the moment interactions with people with a dementia has been appreciated in other contexts (Beard, 2012; Camic et al., 2017) and has been central to the “With All” project. The work of the Elderflowers who use clowning with people with a dementia was also described as innately reliant on improvisation:

Improvisation is the basis of it all. Let’s say clowning and improvisation. Clowning, the way we use it, is where the performers connect with their own vulnerability. Because of that, they connect with the vulnerability of the person with dementia, because often people with dementia are surrounded by people in control.

(Magdalena Schamberger, Elderflowers)

Therefore, to establish a “level playing field” between the clowns and the people they work with, Elderflowers use improvisation. In this context, improvisation is a means of connecting with vulnerability and allows the clowns to relate more closely to people with dementia who often feel out of control and vulnerable themselves.

Improvisation and the absence of a predetermined plan is also associated with a shared uncertainty. This can facilitate the shedding of habitual defences and roles and thus the ability to connect with others. However, improvisation necessarily involves the possibility of everyone taking risks and making mistakes, as noted elsewhere:

The hardest thing to learn is that failure doesn’t matter.

(Frost and Yarrow, 2016, p. xvi)

This understanding contributes to a failure free environment which is especially important for people with a dementia (Swinnen and de Medeiros, 2018). As noted by Kontos et al. (2017) in their discussion of clowning with people with a dementia, spontaneity has further importance in that it:

[…] removes the normative and artificial strictures of structured therapeutic programming […] which may inadvertently thwart creative expression.

(193)

The way in which improvisation can prompt creative openness, “allowing impulses to be free” (as noted by the visual artist Lisa Carter) was apparent for all those interviewed. Although, as noted by one interviewee Dr Andrea Capstick people living with a dementia may find it difficult to “think on their feet” and require prompts as part of the improvisation process, the ability to use these flexibly and responsively was stressed. Above all, improvisation facilitates a creative immediacy and inventiveness for all those involved.

The role of structure and the closely associated concept of leadership a dominant theme that emerged from the interviews with artists. This issue overlaps with notions about the producer/consumer distinction (as outlined by Matarasso, 2017) and the extent to which these boundaries should or can be erased. If a co-creative project is a true meeting of equals in which no-one has more authority than anyone else then should there be a pre-determined structure and can there be a “leader”? The theatre director, Gavin Critchon observed that when he is co-creating theatre he makes it clear that although he understands the form, the people he works with have the content and that there is therefore an inherent co-dependency within the process. In this way, the artists provide a flexible framework (a sort of exo-skeleton) but one that can easily be re-configured and so facilitates creative participation:

It is about having a structure, but being also able to stretch and push that structure and respond to the group a little bit as well. It is kind of having that kind of stretch in there.

(Gavin Critchon)

As similarly noted by the dance artist Diane Amans:

You’ve got to have a structure or some starting point but then noticing when someone lights up and being prepared to follow and having the […] confidence and the disposition to actually share the leadership, lead by just being alongside.

The notion of structure emerged as something that should not be rigid but is rather shaped by the group and is characterised by malleability. The suggestion is that this creates a space that is freer and more suited to creative play. Similarly, the idea that leadership can be understood as being “alongside” people and is not necessarily about purposely directing the activity seems integral to co-creativity. This allows for the important possibility of change within the artistic process, something that is particularly relevant when co-creating with people with a dementia:

There is a structure within it, but it also keeps changing […] because some people with dementia’s behaviour may change quite quickly. So […] you have to be very, very flexible.

(Magdalena Schamberger)

The sense that a co-creative endeavour is predicated on and can promote a sense of equality between all those involved was repeatedly stressed by all the artists. This was eloquently expressed by Lisa Carter who said that her co-creative visual arts practice was “like being part of a sentence” with someone. This conjures up the intertwined nature of co-creative practice in which the parts are dependent on the whole for their meaning and therefore have an equal relevance. The understanding that all those involved have “equally useful” things to offer, was cogently expressed in several interviews:

Co-creativity […] will enable us to create something very different than if we were trying to do that on our own.

(Gavin Critchon)

It’s very much with the aim of being a circle of equals. Each person brings something. I think then the essence is that we co-create from the centre of that circle somehow.

(Lucy Payne)

The artists emphasised the different and therefore equal contributions that each individual could make to the process and how combined, this creates something wholly unique. This echoes the idea of “hybridity”, the equal grafting together of many different voices, sounds or gestures, that has been closely associated with artistic co-creativity (Matarasso, 2017). Andrea Capstick noted the growth of trust and community within a group that were co-creating together:

As time went on, the participants that we were working with actually formed into a group, […] they were interested in each other’s films and they were interested in watching them together.

(Andrea Capstick)

In all the interviews, the artists emphasised their awareness that the essence of their work was about exploring and valuing the equality of all human experiences. There was a sense that this acknowledgement of equality was basic to co-creativity and offered a means of meeting with people affirmatively, where they are.

Our practice in With All has been closely informed by the insights offered by these co-creative artists. Hence, improvisation is prioritised as is the salience of play, the gathering is characterised by a flexible structure that can shift and stretch, similarly leadership is shared within the group rather than located within any one individual and the equality of each person’s contribution is central.

The With All project – co-creativity with people with a dementia

The With All project is a ten week co-creative arts project that is currently taking place at the Wellcome Hub. It comprises three musicians, two dancers and two researchers and centrally a group of people living with a dementia and their partners. Due to ethical constraints which preclude the involvement of people with a more “advanced” dementia taking part in research, the project has been divided into two phases. The first four sessions constitute a “Learning phase” and has included people who could not formally give consent. During these sessions numbers of people with a dementia have varied (due to ill-health) but have ranged from four to one. The second four sessions of the project are the “research phase” and include six people with a dementia and their partners.

Co-creativity as we are starting to conceptualise it within the With All project, shares some similarity to the understandings offered by the design and business worlds. In particular the possibility that distinctions can be erased between the “producer” (or in our project, artist) and “consumer” (or participant). However, it fundamentally differs in that the objective is not to co-design a product or work towards a single composition or performance. There is no emphasis on any particular “outcome”, neither is there a definable end-point.

On the contrary, co-creativity with the arts and with people with a dementia is unwaveringly about mutual involvement in an aesthetic process. Improvisation, play and the subsequent creation of a ludic space is central to this process. Play is an activity that has no directed or practical purpose and exists between imagination and the external world. Play has been described as opening up a space of trust and relaxation which then enables a “creative reaching out” (Winnicott, 1971, p. 75), thus play and the sort of creativity it evokes arises from both openness and formlessness. Play has been cogently theorised as essential for emotional and psychological well-being and the maintenance of a sense of self (Winnicott, 1971) but has only relatively recently been recognised as equally crucial for people with a dementia (Swinnen and de Medeiros, 2018).

Co-creativity as we have witnessed within With All, both relies on and produces a series of interdependent moments which could not exist independently. There is no one author and thus an innate equality. Moreover, there is an important recognition within co-creative practices that creative engagement is an interactive and relational activity that creates community. Co-creativity may generate group or individual compositions and moments of self and group expression that are almost but not quite performances but these are intrinsically linked as they emanate and take shape from the whole group. In this way, co-creativity can be understood both as it is synchronous and as something that necessarily takes place in the moment. This sense that there is only one time within the co-creative process is linked with the modality of improvisation (as theorized by Nachmanovitch, 1990).

Although co-creativity relies on collaboration, it must be understood as more than this – primarily because collaboration is an action that tends to also be understood in relation to the making of a product. Neither is co-creation simply participation – because this suggests that there is something unambiguous or pre-existing to join in with. In addition, in participation there is an implicit suggestion of power relationships – in which a “participant” is distinct from those who lead a group. In contrast, co-creation – based as it is on equality, involves more than simply “joining in” with a predefined activity. At the foundation of co-creativity, is the understanding that everyone has something to offer and that even apparent passivity and quiet affects and helps to direct the mutual creation. Thus, co-creativity is an innately democratic and non-hierarchical version of creativity in which the diverse capacities of all those involved are woven into a cohesive creative process.

Conclusion: why co-create with people with a dementia?

As outlined in the introduction, there are a wide range of different approaches for using the arts with people with a dementia. These all have a valuable role and many have accumulated evidence supporting their benefits. However, restrictive understandings of creativity, as well as a socio-political context that has insistently associated arts with health have affected the ways in which arts projects are designed. Moreover, for a diversity of historical and cultural reasons, there has been very little recognition of people with neurocognitive disorders such as dementia, as creative agents. Yet, in other work we have found that even in people receiving palliative care, the desire to connect, communicate and create can be urgent and passionate (Zeilig et al., 2016).

Co-creativity is about the possibility of using the arts together with people to enable a self-making process and the unexpected insights that this may prompt. There is an ability within the co-creative arts for people to explore uncomfortable emotional experiences (Bellass et al., 2018) which are not accessed within more structured arts activities. Co-creativity with people with a dementia generates relational interactions which are not focused on the completion of instrumental tasks (such as eating, dressing, bathing, etc.) or as a means to an end (such as to increase wellbeing or physical agility). Co-creativity using the arts extends an invitation to participate in a shared and playful pursuit that allows unique opportunities for communication, expression and glimpses into people’s interior worlds. These may have therapeutic potential but this is not the goal.

There are certainly thorny issues associated with co-creativity with people with a dementia. After all, the attempt to work truly inclusively with any group of people is fraught with difficulty, perhaps particularly in terms of validating the contribution of each individual and the complexity of power relations. There are further paradoxes that are related to With All also being a research project which aims to capture and even gauge the effect of certain aspects of co-creativity (such as group involvement) with people with a dementia. We are aware that co-creativity is partly understood as a phenomenon that exists along a spectrum of possibility.

Nonetheless, the possibility that co-creativity can challenge the dominant biomedical and social paradigms that associate “dementia” with irretrievable loss and decline by creating opportunities for creative agency inspires the With All project. We are considering the ways in which people with a dementia can co-create as they are embodied, emotional, desiring, creative agents, embedded in relationships and a social context – and also some of the ways that co-creativity challenges this social context. Indeed, this work is motivated by the possibilities that co-creative practice may promote creative expression and a sense of agency for people with a dementia by privileging reciprocity and being alert to the embodied, relational nature of creativity (Kontos, 2005, 2012; Kontos et al., 2017). Using the arts co-creatively also contributes to a broadening of our understanding of “creativity” by demonstrating that it is something more than a definable attribute possessed by certain individuals that results in a specific product. Co-creativity can therefore offer fresh insights into the way in which the arts can engage people with a dementia, the relationship between creativity and dementia and the transformative potential of the arts for those living with a dementia.

References

Bartlett, R. (2014), “Citizenship in action: the lived experiences of citizens with dementia who campaign for social change”, Disability and Society, Vol. 29 No. 8, pp. 1291-304.

Basting, A. (2009), Forget Memory: Creating Better Lives for People with Dementia, John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.

Beard, R.L. (2012), “Art therapies and dementia care: a systematic review”, Dementia, Vol. 11 No. 5, pp. 633-56.

Beghetto, R.A. and Kaufman, J.C. (2007), “Toward a broader conception of creativity: a case for ‘mini-c’ creativity”, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, Vol. 1 No. 2, pp. 73-9.

Bellass, S., Balmer, A., May, V., Keady, J., Buse, C., Capstick, A., Burke, L., Bartlett, R.L. and Hidgson, J. (2018), “Broadening the debate on creativity and dementia: a critical approach”, Dementia (in press).

Branco, R., Quental, J. and Ribeiro, Ó. (2017), “Personalised participation: an approach to involve people with dementia and their families in a participatory design project”, CoDesign, Vol. 13 No. 2, pp. 127-43.

Castora-Binkley, M., Noelker, L., Prohaska, T. and Satariano, W. (2010), “Impact of arts participation on health outcomes for older adults”, Journal of Aging, Humanities, and the Arts, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 352-67.

Camic, P.M., Hulbert, S. and Kimmel, J. (2017), “Museum object handling: a health-promoting community-based activity for dementia care”, Journal of Health Psychology, pp. 1-12, available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105316685899

Cowl, A. and Gaugler, J. (2014), “Efficacy of creative arts therapy in treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: a systematic literature review”, Activities, Adaptation & Aging, Vol. 38 No. 4, pp. 281-330.

Creative Health, APPG (2017), “All party parliamentary group on arts”, Health and Wellbeing (APPG) Inquiry Report, available at: www.artshealthandwellbeing.org.uk/appg-inquiry/ (accessed July 2017).

Crutch, S., Isaacs, R. and Rossor, M. (2001), “Some workmen can blame their tools: artistic change in an individual with Alzheimer’s disease”, The Lancet, Vol. 357 No. 9274, pp. 2129-33.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997), Creativity: The Psychology of Discovery and Invention, Harper Collins, New York, NY.

de Medeiros, K. and Basting, A. (2014), “‘Shall I compare thee to a dose of Donepezil?’ Cultural arts interventions in dementia care research”, The Gerontologist, Vol. 54 No. 3, pp. 344-53.

de Souza, L., Volle, E., Bertoux, M., Czernecki, V., Funkiewiez, A., Allali, G., Leroy, B., Sarazin, M., Habert, M., Dubois, B., Kas, A. and Levy, R. (2010), “Poor creativity in frontotemporal dementia: a window into the neural bases of the creative mind”, Neuropsychologia, Vol. 48 No. 13, pp. 3733-42.

Dodds, R.E., Tseelon, E. and Weitkamp, E.L.C. (2008), “Making sense of scientific claims in advertising. A study of scientifically aware consumers, public understanding of science”, available at: http://pus.sagepub.com/content/17/2/211.full.pdfhtml (accessed 21 June 2016).

Dupuis, S., Kontos, P., Mitchell, G., Jonas-Simpson, C. and Gray, J. (2016), “Re-claiming citizenship through the arts”, Dementia, Vol. 15 No. 3, pp. 358-80.

Fritsch, T., Adams, K.B., Redd, D., Sias, T. and Herrup, K. (2006), “Use of live theatre to increase minority participation in Alzheimer’s disease research”, Alzheimer Disease Association Disorders, Vol. 20 No. 2, pp. 105-11.

Frost, A. and Yarrow, R. (2016), Improvisation in Drama, Theatre and Performance: History, Practice, Theory, Palgrave, London.

Gray, K., Chester Evans, S., Griffiths, A. and Schneider, J. (2017), “Critical reflections on methodological challenge in arts and dementia evaluation and research”, Dementia, available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1471301217734478 (accessed 5 October 2017).

Gretton, C. and Ffytche, D.H. (2014), “Art and the brain: a view from dementia”, International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatriy, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 111-26.

Hara, M. (2011), “Music in dementia care: increased understanding through mixed research methods”, Music & Arts in Action, Vol. 3 No. 2, pp. 34-58.

Hatton, N. (2014), “Re-imagining the care home: a spatially responsive approach to arts practice with older people in residential care”, Research in Drama Education: The Journal of Applied Theatre and Performance, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 355-65.

He, J. (2014), “Continuity and evolution: the idea of ‘co-creativity’ in Chinese Art”, ASIA Network Exchange: A Journal for Asian Studies in the Liberal Arts, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 15-21.

Hughes, J. (2014), How we Think about Dementia: Personhood, Rights, Ethics, the Arts and What they Mean for Care, Jessica Kingsley, London.

Kontos, P.C. (2005), “Embodied selfhood in Alzheimer’s disease: rethinking person-centred care”, Dementia, Vol. 4, pp. 553-70.

Kontos, P.C. (2012), “Rethinking sociability in long-term care: an embodied dimension of selfhood”, Dementia, Vol. 11, pp. 329-46.

Kontos, P. (2018), “Rethinking musicality in dementia as embodied and relational”, Journal of Aging Studies, available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagin2018.01.006

Kontos, P., Miller, K. and Kontos, A. (2017), “Relational citizenship: supporting embodied selfhood and relationality in dementia care”, Sociology of Health and Illness, Vol. 39 No. 2, pp. 182-98.

Kontos, P., Miller, K., Mitchell, G.J. and Stirling-Twist, J. (2015), “Presence redefined:the reciprocal nature of engagement between elder-clowns and persons with dementia”, Dementia, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 46-66.

Ledger, A. and Edwards, J. (2011), “Arts-based research practices in music therapy research: existing and potential developments”, The Arts in Psychotherapy, Vol. 38 No. 5, pp. 312-17.

McMullan, G. and Smiles, S. (2016), Late Style and its Discontents: Essays in Art, Literature and Music, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Matarasso, F. (2017), “Co-creation from blogsite ‘A restless art’”, available at: https://arestlessart.com/2017/03/09/co-creation/ (accessed 9 March 2017).

Mental Health Foundation (2011), “An evidence review of the impact of participatory arts on older people”, available at: www.mentalhealth.org.uk/publications/evidence-review-impact-participatory-arts-older-people (accessed 27 February 2018).

Moss, H. and O’Neill, D. (2013), “The aesthetic and cultural interests of patients attending an acute hospital – a phenomenological study”, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 70 No. 1, pp. 121-9.

Moss, H. and O’Neill, D. (2017), “Narratives of health and illness: arts-based research capturing the lived experience of dementia”, available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1471301217736163 (accessed 12 October 2017).

Nachmanovitch, S. (1990), Free Play: Improvisation in Life and Art, Penguin Putnam Inc., New York, NY.

Osborne, T. (2003), “Against ‘creativity’: a Philistine rant”, Economy and Society, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 507-25.

Palmiero, M., Di Giacomo, D. and Passafiume, D. (2012), “Creativity and dementia: a review”, Cognitive Processing, Vol. 13 No. 3, pp. 193-209.

Plucker, J.A. and Beghetto, R.A. (2004), “Why Creativity is domain general, why it looks domain specific, and why the distinction does not matter”, in Sternberg, R.J., Grigorenko, E.L. and Singer, J.L. (Eds), Creativity: From Potential to Realization, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 153-68.

Pope, R. (2005), Creativity: Theory, History, Practice, Routledge, Oxford.

Prahalad, C.K. and Ramaswamy, V. (2004), The Future of Competition: Co-Creating Unique Value with Customers, Harvard Business School Press, New York, NY.

Rankin, K., Liu, A., Howard, S., Slama, H., Hou, C., Shuster, K. and Miller, B. (2007), “A case-controlled study of altered visual art production in Alzheimer’s and FTLD”, Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, Vol. 20 No. 1, pp. 48-61.

Salisbury, K., Algar, K. and Windle, G. (2011), “Do we know enough about the potential of art programmes to improve the quality of life and well-being of people with dementia?”, Journal of Dementia Care, Vol. 19 No. 3, pp. 33-7.

Searing, C. and Zeilig, H. (2017), “Fine Lines: cosmetic advertising and the perception of ageing beauty”, International Journal of Ageing and Later Life, Vol. 11 No. 1, pp. 1-30.

Schall, A., Tesky, V.A., Adams, A. and Pantel, J. (2017), “Art museum-based intervention to promote emotional well being and improve quality of life in people with dementias: the ARTEMIS project”, Dementia, available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1471301217730451 (accessed 15 September 2017).

Selberg, S. (2015), “Dementia on the canvas: art and the biopolitics of creativity”, in Swinnen, A. and Schweda, M. (Eds), Popularizing Dementia: Public Expressions and Representations of Forgetfulness, Transcipt Verlag, Bielefeld, pp. 137-62.

Sennett, R. (2012), Together: The Rituals, Pleasures & Politics of Cooperation, Penguin, London.

Swinnen, A. and de Medeiros, K. (2018), “‘Play’ and people living with dementia: a humanities-based inquiry of timeslips and the Alzheimer’s poetry project”, The Gerontologist, Vol. 58 No. 2, pp. 261-69, available at: https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnw196

Winnicott, D.W. (1971), Playing and Reality, Routledge, London.

White, M. (2014), Asking the Way – Directions and Misdirections in Arts in Health, Ixia, London.

Williams, R. (1988), Keywords: A Vocabulary of Culture and Society, Harper Collins, London.

Young, R., Tischler, V., Hulbert, S. and Camic, P. (2015), “The impact of viewing and making art on verbal fluency and memory in people with dementia in an art gallery setting”, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, Vol. 9 No. 4, pp. 368-75.

Zeilig, H., Killick, J. and Fox, C. (2014), “The participative arts for people living with a dementia: a critical review”, International Journal of Ageing and Later Life, Vol. 9 No. 1, pp. 7-34.

Zeilig, H., Poland, F., Fox, C. and Killick, J. (2015), “The arts in dementia care education: a developmental study”, Journal of Public Mental Health, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 8-23.

Zeilig, H., West, J., with Spitalfields Music (2016), “Living Arts Project”, available at: www.spitalfieldsmusic.org.uk/events/livingarts/

Further reading

Basting, A. and Killick, J. (2003), The Arts and Dementia Care: A Resource Guide, The National Centre for Creative Aging, New York, NY.

Camic, P., Baker, E. and Tischler, V. (2015), “Theorizing how art gallery interventions impact people with dementia and their caregivers”, The Gerontologist, Vol. 56 No. 6, pp. 1033-41.

Killick, J. and Craig, C. (2012), Creativity and Communication in Persons with Dementia, Jessica Kingsley Publishers, London.

Ludwin, K. and Capstick, A. (2015), “Using participatory video to understand diversity among people with dementia in long-term care”, Journal of Psychological Issues in Organisational Culture, Vol. 5 No. 4, pp. 30-8.

Richards, R. (2010), “Everyday creativity: process and way of life – four key issues”, in Kaufman, J.C. and Sternberg, R.J. (Eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity, Cambridge University Press, New York, NY, pp. 185-215.

Windle, G., Gregory, S., Howson-Griffiths, T., Newman, A., O’Brian, D. and Goulding, A. (2017), “Exploring the theoretical foundations of visual art programmes for people living with dementia”, Dementia, available at: https://doi.org/10.1177/1471301217726613 ((accessed 15 September 2017).

Young, R., Camic, P.M. and Tischler, V. (2016), “The impact of community-based arts and health interventions on cognition in people with dementia: a systematic literature review”, Aging and Mental Health, Vol. 20 No. 4, pp. 337-51.

Acknowledgements

This work is part of the Created Out of Mind research programme. Created Out of Mind was funded as ‘Created Out of Mind: Shaping Perceptions of Dementias, Grant Ref: 200783/Z/16/Z, by the Wellcome Trust as a part of the Hub Award. (Principal Investigator S. Crutch; Core Group: P. Ball, C. Evans, N. Fox, C. Murphy, F. Walsh, J. West, G. Windle, P. Camic).

Corresponding author

Hannah Zeilig can be contacted at: h.zeilig@fashion.arts.ac.uk

About the authors

Hannah Zeilig is Senior Research Fellow at the University of the Arts, London, UK.

Julian West is based at the Royal Academy of Music, London, UK.

Millie van der Byl Williams is based at the University College London Medical School, London, UK.

Related articles