Dysfunctional perceptual antecedent can justify the social orienting deficit in autism spectrum disorder: an eye-tracking study

Cristina Carrozza (Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina, and Institute for Biomedical Research and Innovation, National Research Council (NCR), Messina, Italy)
Rosa Angela Fabio (Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Università degli Studi di Messina, Messina, Italy)

Advances in Autism

ISSN: 2056-3868

Article publication date: 11 December 2020

Issue publication date: 11 December 2020

1198

Abstract

Purpose

Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) show reduced attention to social stimuli. The reasons for these impairments are still being debated by researchers. The aim of this study is to analyse if reduced attention towards social stimuli is determined by initial underlying difficulties in the control of visual attention. Among the variables that could produce these difficulties, the authors considered geometric complexity and typology of geometric figures.

Design/methodology/approach

To test this hypothesis, in this paper, an eye-tracker paradigm was used for assessing visual exploration and recognition memory towards geometric figures (curved vs rectilinear) with two levels of geometric complexity (low and high) in 17 children with ASD matched with 17 children with typical development (TD).

Findings

The results showed that the ASD group seemed indifferent to both the geometric complexity and the typology of figures (curved and rectilinear), whereas the TD group showed higher performances with highly complex and curved geometric figures than with low complex and rectilinear geometric figures.

Research limitations/implications

Because of the chosen research approach, the research results may lack generalizability. Therefore, researchers are encouraged to test the proposed hypotheses further.

Practical implications

This paper includes implications upon the presence of an unspecified visual attention deficit that is present from the early stages of the processing of stimuli.

Social implications

The understanding of this deficit from the early stages of the processing of stimuli can help educators to intervene at an early stage when disturbances in social relationships are starting.

Originality/value

This study contributes to understanding the presence of dysfunctional perceptual antecedents that could determine general difficulties in paying attention to social stimuli in ASD subjects.

Keywords

Citation

Carrozza, C. and Fabio, R.A. (2020), "Dysfunctional perceptual antecedent can justify the social orienting deficit in autism spectrum disorder: an eye-tracking study", Advances in Autism, Vol. 6 No. 4, pp. 289-302. https://doi.org/10.1108/AIA-03-2020-0015

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2020, Cristina Carrozza and Rosa Angela Fabio.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

The ability to select information with sense organs is the basis of human knowledge. Through inputs from the surrounding environment, the individual acquires information regarding reality that is indispensable to interact with it and build more elaborate forms of knowledge, such as thoughts and memories (Parker and Gibson, 1979). As attention is a key component for complex cognitive operations, a deficit in this function could hinder the development of higher order cognitive and social skills. This difficulty of paying attention to stimuli, especially social stimuli (faces), is present, for example, in subjects with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013; Mukherjee et al., 2018). ASD is a complex neurodevelopmental condition characterized by socio-communicative impairments and restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviors and interests (APA, 2013).

Researchers are still debating the reasons for this reduced attention. It is possible that subjects with ASD pay less attention to social stimuli because these are not “interesting and motivating” (Chevallier et al., 2015; Martin et al., 2019) or because these are not a priority in their attention system (Chawarska et al., 2010). Moreover, the difficulty of focusing on social stimuli (Chawarska et al., 2012, 2013; Fabio et al., 2018a, 2018b; Fabio, 2017; Fabio et al., 2011; Lim, 2019; Shic et al., 2011) is likely to be the direct result of greater emphasis on non-social stimuli, including objects related to circumscribed interests and geometric (Sasson and Touchstone, 2014; Pierce et al., 2011; Pierce et al., 2015; Shi et al., 2015; Shaffer et al., 2017). Despite these results, other studies have also shown that basic deficits in ASD can be attributed to general difficulties in the control of visual attention (Bryson et al., 2004; Landry and Bryson, 2004; Elsabbagh and Johnson, 2007; Elsabbagh et al., 2013; Mohammadhasani et al., 2018, 2019; Sacrey et al., 2013).

In this study, the theory explaining the difficulties of visual exploration in ASD is related to the presence of an attention visual impairment that is present from the early stages of visual input processing (Bryson et al., 2004; Landry and Bryson, 2004; Elsabbagh and Johnson, 2007; Elsabbagh et al., 2013; Sacrey et al., 2013). According to this theory, attention difficulties in subjects with ASD and in other pathologies as the subject with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder are not caused by differences in visual exploration of social stimuli but by initial general difficulties in the control of visual attention (Bryson et al., 2004; Castelli et al., 2013; Elsabbagh et al., 2013; Landry and Bryson, 2004; Fabio and Caprì, 2015, 2017; Fabio et al., 2015; Gangemi et al., 2018; Sacrey et al., 2013). More precisely, some specific, dysfunctional perceptual antecedents might determine these difficulties (Carrozza et al., 2019).

Furthermore, As social stimuli (face – facial configurations) are similar in shape to curved figures (e.g. circle, oval), we considered geometric complexity and typology of geometric figures (curved vs rectilinear) among the variables that could cause these difficulties, in this study.

Several studies have found that children with ASD, compared to children with typical development (TD), display early difference in the perception of geometric stimuli (Pierce et al., 2011; Pierce et al., 2015; Shi et al., 2015; Shaffer et al., 2017). Studies have also found that ASD children, compared to TD children, show the following:

In contrast to these results, Shi et al. (2015) have shown that the stimuli used by Pierce et al. (Pierce et al., 2011; Pierce et al., 2015) with subjects of 2–3 years were not adequate with subjects at a later age (4–6 years). To confirm this hypothesis, they presented the stimuli used by Pierce et al. (2011, 2015) to a sample of preschool children. From this study, no statistically significant differences were found between the ASD group and the TD group. Instead, when the same group of subjects (preschool children) were exposed to more complex social stimuli (children engaged in interacting and playing with each other), they showed reduced attention to more complex social stimuli and greater attention to non-social dynamic stimuli (geometric patterns and letters) (Shi et al., 2015).

Regarding the typology of geometric stimuli (curved or rectilinear), few studies have investigated the impact of type of geometric stimuli on subjects with ASD and their effects on some processes such as judgments, decisions and preferences. For example, McPartland et al. (2011) investigated patterns of visual attention to different types of stimuli (faces, three-dimensional objects and geometric figures) in an ASD group of adolescents. The results showed that ASD subjects, compared to TD subjects, looked more closely at the top region of the stimuli irrespective of the category of belonging (social vs non-social) (McPartland et al., 2011). Recently, Chien et al. (2015) have found that ASD subjects, compared to TD subjects, showed attention and mnemonic difficulties with complex geometric stimuli (Chien et al., 2015). A year later, Belin et al. (2017) investigated the effects of simple geometric stimuli (curved and rectilinear) on attention responses of school-aged children with ASD and with TD (Belin et al., 2017). The results showed that TD children, as widely demonstrated in literature, explored curved geometric shapes more, also by attributing positive emotional connotations to these shapes (Salgado‐Montejo et al., 2015; Bertamini et al., 2016; Velasco et al., 2016). Instead, subjects with ASD observed rectilinear geometric shapes longer and perceived them “positively”. In addition, it was found that when group participants with ASD looked at curved stimuli, they attributed a social value to them (probably because they are similar in shape to facial configurations; face = oval) (Belin et al., 2017; Cotter et al., 2017).

Summarizing the above, there are several differences between the ASD and TD groups. In effect, from the first months of life, children with TD show a preference for curved and rounded lines and figures rather than straight or rectilinear ones (Westerman et al., 2012; Van Oel and Van Den Berkhof, 2013; Vartanian et al., 2013; Palumbo et al., 2015; Salgado‐Montejo et al., 2015; Bertamini et al., 2016; Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016; Palumbo and Bertamini, 2016; Velasco et al., 2016; Cotter et al., 2017). Preference for curved features in children with TD is because of a sort of natural coupling between perceptual features and sensorimotor processes that are attuned to curved configurations.

In a current review, Gómez-Puerto et al. (2016) defined a general framework that explained the preference for curvature considering four explanatory mechanisms: eye movement, specific neural activity, gestalt principles and an appraisal approach (Gallese, 2016; Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016; Belin et al., 2017; Cotter et al., 2017).

With reference to the first mechanism, children preferred curved lines because they allow them to preserve cognitive load, and also because curved lines are related to the ease of eye motion (Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016). Also, some movements are preferred, as they generated fluency and comfort (Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016).

In terms of neurophysiology of the visual system, Fantz and Miranda (1975) showed that, in the early days of life, neonates focused longer on curved stimuli than on rectilinear stimuli (Fantz and Miranda, 1975; cit in Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016). They hypothesized that this early preference is because of cortical cell activity which increases on seeing curved lines (Hubel and Wiesel, 1968; cited in Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016).

Regarding Gestalt principles, a curve can be considered a set of strongly grouped points (Tonder and Spehar, 2013). Some studies reported that preference for curvature is likely caused by intrinsic characteristics of the lines, which can be described as cases of good continuation or good gestalt (Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016). According to Belin et al. (2017) and Cotter et al. (2017) individuals associate curved lines in positive terms, such as quiet, lazy, graceful and playful, whereas rectilinear lines/shapes are considered agitating, hard or furious.

However, pleasure and interest are distinct responses and many pleasant things are not always interesting to individuals (Graf and Landwehr, 2015). Some studies show that people respond differently to certain low-level stimulus features such as familiarity and complexity. As to the complexity of geometric stimuli, this feature has long been discussed in literature (Nadal et al., 2010; Bertamini et al., 2016). Nadal et al. (2010) suggest that there are three different forms of complexity that contribute to people’s perception of visual complexity:

  1. the amount and variety of elements;,

  2. the way those elements are organized; and

  3. asymmetry (Nadal et al., 2010).

In a recent study, Bertamini et al. (2016) calculated the complexity of some geometric shapes with regard to two parameters: articulation (considered as a set of points in a plan, so that the product of the distances is constant) and shape (curved vs rectilinear). From the results of the study, it emerged that in general people preferred geometric curved shapes to rectilinear ones. With reference to complexity, rectilinear geometric shapes were considered more complex than curved ones. According to researchers, these results are due to different articulation and an “ambiguous” orientation of rectilinear lines. The lack of clear orientation influenced the perception of the complexity of the forms (Bertamini et al., 2016).

According to other research, the complexity of stimuli also affects the memory of these stimuli (Huhmann, 2003; Pallett and MacLeod, 2011; Hyde et al., 2016). Specifically, some studies have found that school-aged children are able to remember more simple stimuli than complex ones (Huhmann, 2003). This result is not in agreement with evidence that has emerged in other studies, where it was found that undergraduates tend to remember more complex stimuli (Pallett and MacLeod, 2011). These controversial results depend on the different age of subjects in the studies and on the different stimuli (Huhmann, 2003; Pallett and MacLeod, 2011; Hyde et al., 2016).

Based on literature, the main aim of this study was to understand if visual impairments regarding social stimuli in ASD subjects derive from an attention deficit that is present in the early stages of stimuli processing. Among the variables that could lead to the attention deficit towards social stimuli, this study considered geometric complexity (high vs low) and typology of geometric figures (curved vs rectilinear). Thus, the specific goal of this paper was to assess whether geometric complexity associated with typology of geometric figures influenced the attention deficit towards social stimuli. In addition, considering the influence that this visual attention deficit may have on the development of some cognitive processes (for example learning and memory), this study also investigated the recognition memory of this specific category of stimuli.

The underlying logic of this work is as follows:

  • If the time of visual exploration and number of recognitions of children with ASD were affected both in figures with low geometric complexity and with high geometric complexity, this may be due to the presence of a non-specific attention visual deficit from the early stages of processing of stimuli (pre-attention phase).

  • If children with ASD looked at and memorized figures with a low geometric complexity (irrespective of typology) better than those of high geometric complexity, then the contributing factor may be the level of complexity (geometric).

  • Finally, if there was a difference in visual exploration and in memory of recognition between curved and rectangular figures, irrespective of complexity (high vs low), this would mean that the underlying factor is not the complexity, but the specific deficiency of visual exploration towards curved figures (similar in form to facial configurations).

Methods

Participants

The ASD group was comprised of 17 children (12 males and 5 females) between 36 to 72 months (M = 63.35, SD = 11.45). They were enrolled through the Institute of Applied Sciences and Intelligent Systems (ISASI) – National Research Council of Italy (CNR) – Messina and Provincial Health Service (ASP), Catania, Italy.

ASD diagnosis was made according to DSM-V criteria (APA, 2013) by an experienced multidisciplinary team including two child psychiatrists and two developmental psychologists.

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule - 2 edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012) was used as part of the diagnostic process.

Developmental quotient was assessed using the Griffiths Mental Development Scales (GMDS) (Griffith et al., 2006). The ASD group (M = 63.35, SD = 11.45) was individually matched for gender and verbal developmental quotients (DQ) with a control group of 17 infants (M = 12, F = 5; M = 53.41; SD = 12.44) with TD. These children were recruited from private schools in Messina and Catania. The same team of experts observed also TD children and found that none of them presented any neurological or psychiatric disorders nor had a first-degree family member with a developmental, learning or neurological disorder. None of these children were on medication. Participant characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of the University. All participants’ parents gave written informed consent.

Apparatus

Infrared eye-tracking recording was performed using SMI Eye Tracking device provided by SensoMotoric Instruments (Teltow, Germany). The SMI system measures the gaze direction of each eye and from these measurements it can be evaluated where on the screen the subject is looking. This system recorded the data of both eyes from the reflection of an infrared light source on the cornea and pupil (Billeci et al., 2016).

During the task, children were seated in a chair, approximately 50–55 cm from a 22-inch flat screen monitor with a remote camera. Distance from the screen and inclination angle of the system were adjusted for each child to obtain good tracking of the eyes. Before starting the experimental task, a five-point calibration sequence run, with recording only, started when at least four points were marked as properly calibrated for each eye. To obtain calibration information, children were first shown a coloured ball that appeared in one of five locations on the screen. If calibration quality was poor or fair for any of these points, the process was repeated three times for each subject. Gaze date were recorded with a sample rate of 120 Hz and accuracy better than 1 degree of visual angle. After the calibration sequence, four arrays were presented on the monitor (Figures 1 and 2). Only during the presentation of the first array, did the experimenter gave the subject the following instruction: “Please, carefully observe the figures on the screen: circle, triangle, oval, and rhombus. Then I'll ask you what you've seen before”. After each slide, a black cross was presented in the centre of the screen to ensure that the children’s gaze was directed to the centre. Each array presentation lasted 10,000 ms. Subsequently, the four recognition memory tasks were presented in sequence. Only during the presentation of the first task did the experimenter ask the subject: “Which of these images have you already seen?” (The child could use two types of response: pointing to the stimulus or labelling it.) The correct answer was scored 1; the wrong one, 0. The duration of these tasks depended on the time taken by the subject to provide the answer. When children did not respond, the task timed out after 10 seconds.

Eye tracking measures

For the geometric figures of arrays, Fixation Time (FT) was evaluated, that is the sum of the fixation durations inside the area of interest (AOI). On the other hand, for the stimuli of recognition memory tasks, the sum of the correct recognized items (for each: 0 = wrong, 1 = correct) was considered.

Stimuli

In this study, a task was created with reference to the studies by Bertamini et al. (2016) and Belin et al. (2017). Specifically, with regard to the variable typology of stimuli, in particular the category of curved stimuli, we used curved stimuli presented in the study by Bertamini et al. (2016). Unlike in that study, the eye-tracker paradigm was used (Falck-Ytter et al., 2013; Guillon et al. al., 2014; Chita-Tegmark, 2016) to locate and characterize the subtle variations in visual attention of subjects with ASD to the stimuli presented. In addition, to evaluate the different responses of the two groups (ASD vs. TD) compared to the two typologies of stimuli (curved vs. rectilinear), rectilinear stimuli were also included in this study (Belin et al., 2017). In relation to the geometric complexity parameter (high vs. low), all the studies that conceptualized geometric complexity in terms of quantity of intrinsic elements, orientation, position, articulation and size of geometric (curved and rectilinear) figure were considered (Zhao and Stough, 2005; Wang et al., 2008; Plemenos and Miaoulis, 2009; Nadal et al., 2010; Bertamini et al., 2016). According to these researchers, the level of geometric complexity (high vs. low) depends on the number of visible elements, the area of each element, and the distance and orientation of each element (Zhao and Stough, 2005; Wang et al., 2008; Plemenos and Miaoulis, 2009; Nadal et al., 2010; Bertamini et al., 2016).

In accordance with these studies, in a first phase, eight geometric figures with low complexity (4 rectilinear figures: square, triangle, rhombus and rectangle; 4 curved figures: circle, oval, infinite and cornet) were produced. Subsequently, to create the same figures with high complexity, denseness/area were added to the eight geometric shapes (Anobile et al., 2015; Anobile et al., 2016; Cicchini et al., 2016;) each of them having the same small size (Tables 2 and 3). The geometric figures (n = 16) were inserted into randomly assigned arrays (n = 4) (Figure 1). The array submission order was randomized and counterbalanced for all participants in the study.

Additionally, for the recognition memory, four recognition memory tasks were entered after each array. Each task contained a target stimulus and a distracting stimulus.

The task (calibration, arrays, fixing points and recognition memory tasks) had an average duration of approximately 4 minutes.

In order to extrapolate gaze data of each subject, rectangular AOIs were defined in the array (5 AOIs: 4 for the figures and 1 for background), in the fixing point (1 AOI) and in the recognition memory tasks (2 AOIs: 1 for the figure and 1 for distractor). The AOIs were the same size.

Statistical analyses

Data were analysed using SPSS Version 24.0 for MacOS. Descriptive statistics on the characteristics of the experimental sample were presented separately for subjects with ASD and subjects with TD. Comparisons between the two groups were performed using independent samples t-tests. The descriptive statistics of the two groups show that there are significant differences only with respect to chronological age, t (33) = 2.37, p < .* 02, while for the quartile spread (QS) no statistically significant effects occur.

Regarding the analysis of parameters extracted from the eye-tracking, the following tests were used: repeated measures analysis of variance and t-tests for paired samples. The data were analysed considering two parameters: visual exploration (FT) and recognition memory (sum of correct response) of both groups (ASD and TD). Alpha level was set to 0.05 for all statistical tests. For all statistical tests, in cases of significant effects, the effect size of the test was reported.

Results

The results are presented firstly in relation to visual exploration parameters and after in relation to recognition memory. Before analyzing these two parameters, the FT of the four matrices of both groups was evaluated. An analysis of variance was performed: 2 (Groups: ASD and TD) × 4 (Order of the arrays: 1st, 2nd, 3rd and 4th). Based on the results, it was found that the order of the arrays factor has no significant effects. These data indicated that each of the four matrices was explored for the same time by both groups.

Visual exploration

With respect to the first parameter (FT), repeated measures analysis of variance with a between subject factor and two within subject factors was performed: 2 (Groups: ASD vs TD) × 2 (Geometric Complexity: High vs Low) × 2 (Typology of Figures: Curved vs Rectilinear). The Group factor showed significant effects, F (1, 32) = 12, 93, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.92. This indicated that subjects with ASD, compared to subjects with TD, fixated figures within the arrays for less time. The variable Geometric Complexity also had significant effects, F (1, 32) = 12.92, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.91. This indicated that when geometric complexity was high, both groups tended to fixate the stimuli for longer time than when geometric complexity was low (Table 2).

Regarding Typology of Figure (curved and rectilinear), statistically significant differences were found, F (1, 32) = 9, 92, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.91. The interaction Group × Typology of Figures also showed significant effects, F (1, 32) = 3.71, p <0.005, η2p = 0.92. This indicated that subjects with ASD fixated both types of shapes for the same time; instead, subjects with TD looked at curved forms for a longer time than rectilinear ones. The interaction Groups × Complexity × Typology of Figures showed significant effect, F (1, 32) = 2.27, p < 0.05, η2p = 0.68. When geometric complexity increased, subjects with ASD, compared to subjects with TD, reduced fixations towards curved stimuli (see Figure 3).

Recognition memory

Regarding recognition memory, a repeated measures analysis of variance was performed: 2 (Groups: ASD vs TD) × 2 (Geometric Complexity: High vs Low) × 2 (Typology of Figures: Curved vs Rectilinear). The parameters considered were: FT towards the target stimulus and the distracting stimulus and Correctness of responses.

With reference to the first parameter (FT), no statistically significant differences were observed in either group.

Instead, in relation to the second parameter (Correctness of the answers), the variable ‘Group’ showed significant effects, F (1, 32) = 3.08, p <0.005, η2p = 0.83. This indicated that subjects with ASD, compared to subjects with TD, generally tended to remember the forms seen earlier to a lesser extent. This result was consistent with that of visual exploration. Specifically, subjects with ASD explored and recalled less rectilinear and curved shapes than the TD group (Table 3). Finally, recognition did not seem to be influenced by the other variables considered.

Discussion

The aim of this paper is to understand whether the visual impairment of visual exploration towards social stimuli (faces) in the development of ASD symptoms derives from an attention deficit present from the early stages of processing of stimuli. Among the variables that could cause the attention deficit to social and non-social stimuli, geometric complexity and typology of geometric shapes (curved vs. rectilinear) were evaluated in this study. The basic idea is to investigate whether the perception of geometric complexity linked to curved and rectilinear (simple and complex) figures influences the deficit towards social stimuli in subjects with ASD.

The data were analysed considering two parameters: visual exploration and recognition memory.

Visual exploration results indicate that both variables considered (geometric complexity and typology of geometric figures) were statistically significant. Specifically, as far as the typology of figures are concerned, the data confirm what emerged in literature, namely that subjects with TD compared to subjects with ASD look more at curved than at rectilinear lines and figures (Westerman et al., 2012; Van Oel and Van Den Berkhof, 2013; Vartanian et al., 2013; Palumbo, Ruta and Bertamini; 2015; Salgado‐Montejo et al., 2015; Bertamini et al., 2016; Gómez-Puerto et al., 2016; Palumbo and Bertamini, 2016; Velasco et al., 2016; Cotter et al., 2017). In relation to the complexity variable, the results showed that subjects with ASD seem indifferent to the complexity or to the typology of figures (curved or rectilinear) (Chien et al., 2015; Belin et al., 2017).

Regarding recognition memory, the results show that subjects with ASD, compared to subjects with TD, generally tend to recall previously seen rectilinear and curved shapes to a lesser extent. These data were consistent with visual exploration results. Thus, subjects with ASD did not explore or remember rectilinear and curved shapes as well as the TD group.

With reference to the hypotheses expressed in the introduction, results confirmed the three points analysed (a, b, c). Subjects with ASD a) generally had low indexes of visual exploration on all stimuli; b) they seem indifferent to both geometric complexity c) and to type of figures (curved and rectilinear). Since all the variables considered reflect an inclusive, rather than exclusive, model of deficit, it is possible to hypothesize the presence of an unspecified visual attention deficit from the early stages of processing of stimuli (c).

Following from the above, these results could be interpreted as perceptual functional antecedents that can explain the visual exploration deficit to social stimuli. Specifically, if subjects with ASD do not pay attention to curve and complex figures, this could explain their difficulty in paying attention to faces that are in themselves curved and complex stimuli.

Despite the fact that functional perceptions of the lack of orientation towards social stimuli in subjects with ASD have been investigated, the limit of the present work is the use of “abstract stimuli” (geometric figures). Therefore, future research should focus on the following aspects: a) evaluation of visual exploration and recognition memory of more ecological and naturalistic simple and complex stimuli; b) the study of simple and complex stimuli from other sensory domains (e.g. auditory) and finally c) analysis of complexity in wider samples and with subjects from different age groups (toddler, school-aged children, adolescent and adult). Because of the chosen research approach, another limitation of this research is the lack generalizability of the results. Therefore, researchers are encouraged to further test the proposed hypotheses in the future.

Conclusion

This study contributes to understanding the presence of dysfunctional perceptual antecedents that could determine general difficulties in paying attention to social stimuli in ASD subjects. Understanding this deficit from the early stages of the processing of stimuli in children with ASD has important implications for clinical and therapeutic aspects. With regard to the clinical aspect, this knowledge on how children with ASD in early stages explore some stimuli (social stimuli) allows to investigate and study specific difficulties in the control of visual attention and the consequences that these difficulties might have on important cognitive development (such as learning and memory). Moreover, from a therapeutic point of view, this knowledge could have important consequences for the timeliness of the intervention (early, intensive intervention allows to exploit the brain plasticity which is typical of the first three years of life) and for the setting of therapeutic work (such as typology of intervention, areas to be strengthened and which methodology to use based on the difficulties and the functioning capacity of the subject).

The manuscript received ethics approval and consent to participate. The datasets during and/or analysed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. The authors declare that they have no competing interests. The author received no funding.

CC was a major contributor in writing the manuscript, RAF analysed, interpreted and wrote all the results. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Figures

Array with geometric figures (curved vs rectilinear)

Figure 1

Array with geometric figures (curved vs rectilinear)

Position of the subject in front of the eyetracker

Figure 2

Position of the subject in front of the eyetracker

Fixation Time (FT) of the Interaction Group × Complexity × Typology of figures

Figure 3

Fixation Time (FT) of the Interaction Group × Complexity × Typology of figures

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample

ASD (N = 17)
Mean (SD)
TD (N = 17)
Mean (SD)
t (33)-values p-values
Age (months) 63.35 (11.45) 53.41 (12.44) 2.37 0.02
Gender (M:F) 12:5 12:5 --- ---
DQ (GDMS) 52.88 (10.70) 56.64 (13.10) 1.00 0.31
Language DQ 57.47 (11.23) 62.94 (13.11) 1.17 0.25
Manual eyepiece DQ 56.76 (13.82) 53.76 (16.07) 0.49 0.62
Performance DQ 60.64 (15.34) 64.11 (16.82) 0.65 0.51
Practical reasoning DQ 37.05 (14.86) 46.36 (18.65) 1.6 0.08
ADOS – 2 Total 14.6 (5.0) --- --- ---

Means (SD) of the fixation time of curved and rectilinear figures

Stimuli ASD
Means (SD)
TD
Means (SD)
Low complexity
Square 1.52 (2.09) 2.23 (1.67)
Triangle 1.23 (1.82) 3.29 (2.88)
Rectangle 0.76 (1.09) 2.35 (1.90)
Rhombus 2.17 (2.48) 3.52 (3.16)
Circle 1.11 (1.21) 2.17 (1.77)
Oval 0.76 (1.09) 2.76 (2.38)
Cornet 1.64 (2.05) 2.94 (2.46)
Infinity 1.00 (1.62) 2.82 (2.57)
High complexity
Square 1.29 (1.49) 1.76 (2.27)
Triangle 1.70 (2.02) 2.94 (2.01)
Rectangle 1.29 (1.57) 3.29 (2.61)
Rhombus 1.82 (2.62) 2.88 (2.91)
Circle 2.29 (3.31) 4.17 (3.02)
Oval 2.17 (2.50) 2.00 (1.45)
Cornet 1.88 (1.83) 6.11 (3.31)
Infinity 1.88 (1.83) 6.11 (3.31)

Means (SD) of the number of correct responses

Stimuli ASD
Means (SD)
TD
Means (SD)
Low complexity
Rectilinear 2.18 (1.55) 2.62 (1.66)
Curved 2.18 (1.32) 2.75 (1.43)
High complexity
Rectilinear 1.81 (1.32) 3.06 (0.99)
Curved 1.75 (1.23) 2.37 (0.95)

References

Anobile, G., Cicchini, G.M. and Burr, D.C. (2016), “Number as a primary perceptual attribute: a review”, Perception, Vol. 45 Nos 1/2, pp. 5-31.

Anobile, G., Turi, M., Cicchini, G.M. and Burr, D.C. (2015), “Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects”, Journal of Vision, Vol. 15 No. 5, pp. 4-4.

APA (2013), “Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders”, (DSM-5th), American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.

Belin, L., Henry, L., Destays, M., Hausberger, M. and Grandgeorge, M. (2017), “Simple shapes elicit different emotional responses in children with autism spectrum disorder and neurotypical children and adults”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 8, pp. 12-23.

Bertamini, M., Palumbo, L., Gheorghes, T.N. and Galatsidas, M. (2016), “Do observers like curvature or do they dislike angularity?”, British Journal of Psychology, Vol. 107 No. 1, pp. 154-178.

Billeci, L., Narzisi, A., Campatelli, G., Crifaci, G., Calderoni, S., Gagliano, A. and Muratori, F. (2016), “Disentangling the initiation from the response in joint attention: an eye-tracking study in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders”, Translational Psychiatry, Vol. 6 No. 5, p. e808.

Bryson, S.E., Landry, R., Czapinski, P., McConnell, B., Rombough, V. and Wainwright, A. (2004), “Autistic spectrum disorders: causal mechanisms and recent findings on attention and emotion”, International Journal of Special Education, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 14-22.

Carrozza, C., Fabio, R.A., Pugliese, A. and Martino, G. (2019), “Simple and complex images: operationalization and first calibration”, APMB - Atti Della Accademia Peloritana Dei Pericolanti – Classe di Scienze Medico Biologiche, Vol. 23, pp.36-67, doi: 10.6092/1828-6550.

Castelli, I., Antonietti, A., Fabio, R.A., Lucchini, B. and Marchetti, A. (2013), “Do rett syndrome persons possess theory of mind? Some evidence from not-treated girls”, Life Span and Disability, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 157-168.

Chawarska, K., Macari, S. and Shic, F. (2012), “Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism”, Yale University School of Medicine, Child Study Center J Child Psychol Psychiatry, August, Vol. 53 No. 8, pp. 33-45.

Chawarska, K., Macari, S. and Shic, F. (2013), “Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with ASD”, Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 74 No. 3, pp. 195-203.185.

Chawarska, K., Volkmar, F. and Klin, A. (2010), “Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders”, Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 67 No. 2, pp. 178-185.

Chevallier, C., Parish-Morris, J., McVey, A., Rump, K.M., Sasson, N.J., Herrington, J.D. and Schultz, R.T. (2015), “Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: stimulus type matters”, Autism Research, Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 620-628.

Chien, Y.L., Gau, S.F., Shang, C.Y., Chiu, Y.N., Tsai, W.C. and Wu, Y.Y. (2015), “Visual memory and sustained attention impairment in youths with autism spectrum disorders”, Psychological Medicine, Vol. 45 No. 11, pp. 2263-2273.

Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016), “Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies”, Research in Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 48, pp. 79-93.

Cicchini, G.M., Anobile, G. and Burr, D.C. (2016), “Spontaneous perception of numerosity in humans”, Nature Communications, Vol. 7 No. 1, pp. 12-15.

Cotter, K.N., Silvia, P.J., Bertamini, M., Palumbo, L. and Vartanian, O. (2017), “Curve appeal: exploring individual differences in preference for curved versus angular objects”, i-Perception, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 34-38.

Elsabbagh, M., Fernandes, J., Webb, S.J., Dawson, G., Charman, T. and Johnson, M.H. and British Autism Study of Infant Siblings Team (2013), “Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood”, Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 74 No. 3, pp. 189-194.

Elsabbagh, M. and Johnson, M.H. (2007), “Infancy and autism: progress, prospects, and challenge”, Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 164, pp. 76-82.

Fabio, R.A. and Caprì, T. (2015), “Autobiographical memory in ADHD subtypes”, Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, Vol. 40 No. 1, pp. 26-36.

Fabio, R.A. (2017), “The study of automatic and controlled processes in ADHD: a reread and a new proposal”, Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 5, pp. 1-8.

Fabio, R.A. and Caprì, T. (2017), “The executive functions in a sample of Italian adults with ADHD: attention, response inhibition and planning/organization”, Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, Vol. 5 No. 3, pp. 1-17.

Fabio, R.A. and Urso, M. (2014), “The analysis of attention network in ADHD, attention problems and typically developing subjects”, Life Span and Disability, Vol. 17 No. 2, pp. 199-221.

Fabio, R.A., Castriciano, C. and Rondanini, A. (2015), “ADHD: auditory and visual stimuli in automatic and controlled processes”, Journal of Attention Disorders, Vol. 19 No. 9, pp. 771-778.

Fabio, R.A., Oliva, P. and Murdaca, A.M. (2011), “Systematic and emotional contents in overselectivity processes in autism”, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. 575-583.

Fabio, R.A., Magaudda, C., Caprì, T., Towey, G.E. and Martino, G. (2018a), “Choice behavior in rett syndrome: the consistency parameter”, Life Span and Disability, Vol. 21 No. 1, pp. 47-62.

Fabio, R.A., Caprì, T., Mohammadhasani, N., Gangemi, A., Gagliano, A. and Martino, G. (2018b), “Frequency bands in seeing and remembering: comparing ADHD and typically developing children”, Neuropsychological Trends, Vol. 24 No. 24, pp. 7-116, doi: 10.7358/neur-2018-024.

Falck-Ytter, T., Bolte, S. and Gredeback, G. (2013), “Eye tracking in early autism research”, Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, Vol. 5 No. 1, p. 28.

Fantz, R.L. and Miranda, S.B. (1975), “Newborn infant attention to form of contour”, Child Development, Vol. 46 No. 1, pp. 224-228.

Gallese, V. (2016), “The multimodal nature of visual perception: facts and speculations”, Gestalt Theory, Vol. 38 Nos 2/3.

Gangemi, A., Caprí, T., Fabio, R.A., Puggioni, P., Falzone, A.M. and Martino, G. (2018), “Transcranial direct current stimulation (TDCS) and cognitive empowerment for the functional recovery of diseases with chronic impairment and genetic etiopathogenesis”, Advances in Genetics Research, Vol. 45, pp. 179-196.

Gómez-Puerto, G., Munar, E. and Nadal, M. (2016), “Preference for curvature: a historical and conceptual framework”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Vol. 9

Graf, L.K. and Landwehr, J.R. (2015), “A dual-process perspective on fluency-based aesthetics: the pleasure-interest model of aesthetic liking”, Personality and Social Psychology Review, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 395-410.

Griffith, R., Luiz, D. & Association for Research in Infant and Child Development (2006), “Griffiths mental development scales, extended revised: GMDS-ER; two to eight years”.

Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. (1968), “Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex”, The Journal of Physiology, Vol. 195 No. 1, pp. 215-243.

Huhmann, B.A. (2003), “Visual complexity in banner ads: the role of color, photography, and animation”, Visual Communication Quarterly, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 10-17.

Hyde, D.C., Flom, R. and Porter, C.L. (2016), “Behavioral and neural foundations of multisensory face-voice perception in infancy”, Developmental Neuropsychology, Vol. 41 No. 5-8, pp. 273-292.

Landry, R. and Bryson, S.E. (2004), “Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism”, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 45 No. 6, pp. 1115-1122.

Lim, J. (2019), “Emotion regulation and intervention in adults with autism spectrum disorder: a synthesis of the literature”, Advances in Autism, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 48-62.

Lord, C., Petkova, E., Hus, V., Gan, W., Lu, F., Martin, D.M., Ousley, O. and Risi, S. (2012), “A multisite study of the clinical diagnosis of different autism spectrum disorders”, Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 69 No. 3, pp. 306-313.

McPartland, J.C., Webb, S.J., Keehn, B. and Dawson, G. (2011), “Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 41 No. 2, pp. 148-157.

Martin, N., Milton, D., Krupa, J., Brett, S., Bulman, K., Callow, D., Copeland, F., Cunningham, L., Ellis, W., Harvey, T., Moranska, M., Roach, R. and Wilmot, S. (2019), “The sensory school: working with teachers, parents and pupils to create good sensory conditions”, Advances in Autism, Vol. 5 No. 2, pp. 131-140.

Minshew, N.J. and Goldstein, G. (2001), “The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism”, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 42 No. 8, pp. 1095-1101.

Mohammadhasani, N., Caprì, T., Nucita, A., Iannizzotto, G. and Fabio, R.A. (2019), “Atypical visual scan path affects remembering in ADHD”, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, Vol. 26 No. 6, doi: 10.1017/S135561771900136X.

Mohammadhasani, N., Fardanesh, H., Hatami, J., Mozayani, N. and Fabio, R.A. (2018), “The pedagogical agent enhances mathematics learning in ADHD students”, Education and Information Technologies, Vol. 23 No. 6, pp. 2299-2308, doi: 10.1007/s10639-018-9710-x.

Mukherjee, R., Nawaz, M. and Joseph, T. (2018), “Assessing the complexity of adult ASD cases across three areas in Southern England: a service evaluation”, Advances in Autism, Vol. 4 No. 1, pp. 2-9.

Nadal, M., Munar, E., Marty, G. and Cela-Conde, C.J. (2010), “Visual complexity and beauty appreciation: explaining the divergence of results”, Empirical Studies of the Arts, Vol. 28 No. 2, pp. 173-191.

Pallett, P.M. and MacLeod, D.I. (2011), “Seeing faces as objects: no face inversion effect with geometrical discrimination”, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, Vol. 73 No. 2, pp. 504-520.

Palumbo, L. and Bertamini, M. (2016), “The curvature effect: a comparison between preference tasks”, Empirical Studies of the Arts, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 35-52.

Palumbo, L., Ruta, N. and Bertamini, M. (2015), “Comparing angular and curved shapes in terms of implicit associations and approach/avoidance responses”, PloS One, Vol. 10 No. 10, p. e0140043.

Parker, S.T. and Gibson, K.R. (1979), “A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 2 No. 3, pp. 367-381.

Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R. and Desmond, J. (2011), “Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism”, Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 68 No. 1, September. 113.

Pierce, K., Marinero, S., Hazin, R., McKenna, B., Barnes, C.C. and Malige, A. (2015), “Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity”, Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 79 No. 8, pp. 657-666.

Plemenos, D. and Miaoulis, G. (2009), Visual Complexity and Intelligent Computer Graphics Techniques Enhancements (Vol. 200), Springer, New York.

Sacrey, L.A.R., Bryson, S.E. and Zwaigenbaum, L. (2013), “Prospective examination of visual attention during play in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorder: a longitudinal study from 6 to 36 months of age”, Behavioural Brain Research, Vol. 256, pp. 441-450.

Salgado‐Montejo, A., Tapia Leon, I., Elliot, A.J., Salgado, C.J. and Spence, C. (2015), “Smiles over frowns: when curved lines influence product preference”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 32 No. 7, pp. 771-781.

Sasson, N.J. and Touchstone, E.W. (2014), “Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 44 No. 3, pp. 584-592.

Shaffer, R.C., Pedapati, E.V., Shic, F., Gaietto, K., Bowers, K., Wink, L.K. and Erickson, C.A. (2017), “Brief report: diminished gaze preference for dynamic social interaction scenes in youth with autism spectrum disorders”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 47 No. 2, pp. 506-513.

Shi, L., Zhou, Y., Ou, J., Gong, J., Wang, S., Cui, X. and Luo, X. (2015), “Different visual preference patterns in response to simple and complex dynamic social stimuli in preschool-aged children with”, Plos One”, , Vol. 10 No. 3, p. e0122280.

Shic, F., Bradshaw, J., Klin, A., Scassellati, B. and Chawarska, K. (2011), “Limited activity monitoring in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder”, Brain Research, Vol. 1380, pp. 246-254.

Tonder, G.V. and Spehar, B. (2013), “The aesthetic appeal of visual qualities”, Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology: Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance, in Handbook of Experimental Phenomenology: Visual Perception of Shape, Space and Appearance, John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, pp. 395-414.

Van Oel, C.J. and van den Berkhof, F.D. (2013), “Consumer preferences in the design of airport passenger areas”, Journal of Environmental Psychology, Vol. 36, pp. 280-290.

Vartanian, O., Navarrete, G., Chatterjee, A., Fich, L.B., Leder, H., Modroño, C., Nadal, M., Rostrup, N. and Skov, M. (2013), “Impact of contour on aesthetic judgments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 110 No. Supplement_2, pp. 10446-10453.

Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Elliot, A.J., Woods, A.T., Alvarado, J. and Spence, C. (2016), “The shapes associated with approach/avoidance words”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 40 No. 5, pp. 689-702.

Wang, D., Belyaev, A., Saleem, W. and Seidel, H.P. (2008), “Shape complexity from image similarity”.

Westerman, S.J., Gardner, P.H., Sutherland, E.J., White, T., Jordan, K., Watts, D. and Wells, S. (2012), “Product design: preference for rounded versus angular design elements”, Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 29 No. 8, pp. 595-605.

Zhao, Z. and Stough, R.R. (2005), “Measuring similarity among various shapes based on geometric matching”, Geographical Analysis, Vol. 37 No. 4, pp. 410-422.

Further reading

Elsabbagh, M. and Johnson, M.H. (2016), “Autism and the social brain: the first-year puzzle”, Biological Psychiatry, Vol. 80 No. 2, pp. 94-99.

Fabio, R.A., Martinazzoli, C. and Antonietti, A. (2005), “Construction and standardization of the RARS (rett assessment rating scale) tool”, [Costruzione e Standardizzazione Dello Strumento RARS (Rett Assessment Rating Scale)] Life Span and Disability, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 34-45.

Ruta, N., Palumbo, L. and Bertamini, M. (2014), Comparing Angular and Smoothed Polygons. Exploring the Link between Preference, Response Time and Contour Integration, IAEA, New York, NY.

Corresponding author

Rosa Angela Fabio can be contacted at: rafabio@unime.it

About the authors

Cristina Carrozza is based at the Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Messina, and Institute for Biomedical Research and Innovation, National Research Council (NCR), Messina, Italy

Rosa Angela Fabio is based at the Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Università degli Studi di Messina, Messina, Italy

Related articles