Being unwanted and other very early predictors of adult psychopathy

Georgia Zara (Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy)
Henriette Bergstrøm (School of Psychology, College of Health, Psychology and Social Care, University of Derby, Derby, UK)
David P. Farrington (Institute of Criminology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK)

Journal of Criminal Psychology

ISSN: 2009-3829

Article publication date: 13 March 2023

Issue publication date: 5 January 2024

2258

Abstract

Purpose

This paper aims to present new evidence from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development (CSDD) showing the extent to which obstetric (e.g. abnormal birth weight, confinement at birth, severe abnormality of pregnancy, etc.) and early childhood and family factors (illegitimate child, unwanted conception, family overcrowding, etc.) have predictive effects on psychopathic traits measured later in life at age 48 years.

Design/methodology/approach

Data collected in the CSDD are analysed. This is a prospective longitudinal study of 411 London men from age 8 to age 61 years.

Findings

The results suggest that none of the obstetric problems were predictive of adult psychopathy. However, some other early childhood factors were significant. Unwanted conception (by the mother) was significantly associated with high psychopathy. The likelihood of being an unwanted child was higher when the mother was younger (19 years or less), and when the child was illegitimate. The poor health of the mother and living in an overcrowded family were also significant in predicting psychopathy in adulthood, as well as both psychopathic personality (F1) and psychopathic behaviour (F2).

Originality/value

These findings suggest the influence of very early emotional tensions and problematic social background in predicting psychopathic traits in adulthood (at age 48 years). They also emphasise the importance of investigating further the very early roots of psychopathic traits.

Keywords

Citation

Zara, G., Bergstrøm, H. and Farrington, D.P. (2024), "Being unwanted and other very early predictors of adult psychopathy", Journal of Criminal Psychology, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 1-15. https://doi.org/10.1108/JCP-01-2023-0002

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023, Georgia Zara, Henriette Bergstrøm and David P. Farrington.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Criminology has traditionally failed to take the full life course into consideration when attempting to explain antisocial and criminal behaviour. As a result, relevant insights into criminal trajectories and precursors to antisociality and criminality have been neglected (Farrington, 2005). This also applies to the study of psychopathy, where much of the knowledge of the development of psychopathy comes from cross-sectional studies that generally only target a specific period in the life course (e.g. adulthood) (Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2018, 2022; Zara et al., 2021). Research on precursors and correlates of psychopathy has yielded inconsistent results (De Brito, et al., 2021; Lynam et al., 2007), and some (Thomson, 2019) have argued that these inconsistences could be because of the nature of psychopathy. The “elusive category” that Lewis (1974) referred to when talking about psychopathy continues to remain a relevant terminology even after decades of research on the construct (Vitale, 2022).

Psychopathy is a personality disorder that is characterised by a constellation of affective, interpersonal, lifestyle and antisocial symptoms that cluster together (Hare, 2003). The “prototypical” psychopath is charming and manipulative, lacks empathy and has shallow affect. The combination of all these features makes what is considered psychopathic personality (or Factor 1). This prototypical psychopath is impulsive, takes advantage of others, is involved in an erratic, irresponsible and antisocial lifestyle and engages often in criminal behaviour. All these prototypical features comprise what is considered psychopathic behaviour (or Factor 2). Though psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) are often considered to be similar because they share a chronic antisocial lifestyle pattern, and clinical research has shown that they are moderately correlated, they are rather distinct disorders (De Brito et al., 2021). The affective and interpersonal features are those that distinguish psychopaths from individuals diagnosed with a broader range of ASPD symptoms as also emphasised in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; see De Brito et al., 2021; Hare and Neumann, 2009).

This distinction seems especially relevant when assessing psychopathy: much reliance on the behavioural expressions of the disorder (such as an erratic and deviant life style, antisocial and criminal behaviour) might lead to an inaccurate assessment or diagnosis (for a critical review see Boduszek and Debowska, 2016). Research shows that affective and interpersonal traits tend to be stable over time (Blonigen et al., 2006) and antisocial and criminal behaviour tends to decline over time (Farrington, 2019b, 2021). Hence, affective and interpersonal deficits are likely to be at the core of psychopathy (Boduszek and Debowska, 2016), whereas a deviant life style and involvement in criminality are possible behavioural manifestations of psychopathy (Skeem and Cooke, 2010). For instance, in a study carried out by Boduszek et al. (2021), different populations (prisoners, community adults, university students and adolescents) were compared on four sets of psychopathy traits (affective responsiveness, cognitive responsiveness, interpersonal manipulation and egocentricity). Findings showed the presence of qualitatively different groups of psychopathy, with prisoners being not more psychopathic than non-prisoners. It is interesting to note that the high psychopathy group for their prisoner and student samples amounted to approximately 7% (Boduszek et al., 2021).

Debate is still ongoing as to how psychopathy develops and how it is affected by early life events, as described in a vast literature (Beaver et al., 2012; Bergstrøm and Farrington, 2018; Boduszek et al., 2019; Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2018, 2021; Glenn, 2018; Jackson and Beaver, 2016; Thomson, 2019; Zara and Farrington, 2016), but there is a clear research gap when it comes to the potential role of obstetric and early infancy factors (Thomson, 2019). This is an important gap to address, as the study of these factors may have implications for how we understand the development of psychopathy, bridging our knowledge between biological, psychological and social factors.

Farrington and Bergstrøm (2021) investigated the extent to which early risk factors, measured at ages 8–10, could predict psychopathy in adulthood, at age 48. They investigated the risk factors which were judged to be most important in the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development (CSDD). They found that the best predictors of Factor 1 were attending a high delinquency rate school, high daring, high hyperactivity and low verbal intelligence, while the best predictors of Factor 2 were high troublesomeness, high daring, high dishonesty and low family income.

In light of these previous findings, the aim of the present paper is to investigate to what extent very early risk factors (e.g. measured at birth) predict psychopathy at age 48. Only a few studies have attempted to investigate which childhood factors predict callous-unemotional behaviour in childhood (Bedford et al., 2017) and psychopathic traits in adolescence (Beaver et al., 2014; Kerig et al., 2012) or adulthood (Glenn et al., 2007; Jackson and Beaver, 2016). However, no previous studies have investigated very early predictors of psychopathy in middle age.

Identifying precursors of psychopathy: from the early beginnings

Pregnancy and infancy are crucial for individual development and later social adjustment (Olds et al., 1998) because they are closely intertwined with attachment (Vaughn et al., 2008). Attachment is a basic human need that fosters connection to others and involves a close relationship between the infant and the primary caregiver (Beckett and Taylor, 2010). John Bowlby (1969) defined attachment as a unique relationship between the infant and her/his main caregiver (usually the mother) that is the foundation for further (healthy or unhealthy) development. The child’s sense of trust or distrust towards the world depends on it (Gurol and Palot, 2012). Research (Brandon et al., 2009; Cranley, 1981) shows that attachment starts at a prenatal level. In other words, prenatal attachment creates the foundation for developing the emotional closeness and psychological bond between the pregnant mother and her unborn infant (Bretherton and Waters, 1985; Condon and Corkindale, 1997). Circumstances surrounding the pregnancy and birth (obstetric variables) can also influence later life outcomes of the child. Some can be indicators of neuropsychological problems (Tibbetts and Piquero, 1999), others can impact upon the emotional stability of the individual (Barker et al., 2011), and others can influence social adjustment to life (Olds et al., 1998; Van den Bergh et al., 2005). For example, stress during pregnancy has been related to later psychopathic traits of the child (Gao et al., 2016).

Studies conducted on human beings have revealed that care deprivation and family stressors, especially early in life, have a major effect on right brain regulation (Ishikawa and Raine, 2003) and on emotional and mental development (Mack et al., 2011). Behavioural effects could be seen in weakened social interactions, school drop-out, conduct disorders (Loeber et al., 1998), aggressive and hostile behaviours during childhood (Dekleyn and Greenberg, 2008), behavioural disorders during adolescence and experiences of drug abuse and social maladjustment during adulthood (Sroufe, 2005).

Results have however been mixed. For example, unwanted pregnancy might contribute to an increased risk for later criminal behaviour of the offspring (Donahue and Levitt, 2000), albeit its effect has more recently been contested (Shoesmith, 2015). Low birth weight has been found to be a positive predictor for general criminality (Tibbetts and Piquero, 1999), and for internalising problems (Boyle et al., 2011) but not for psychopathy.

Lalumière et al. (2001) conducted two comprehensive studies on the role of developmental problems in psychopathy. Most relevant to the current discussion is the first study of 800 psychopathic (n = 157) and non-psychopathic (n = 643) offenders, where a wide range of obstetric problems were recorded. The most common obstetric problems were “mother older than 35 at subject’s birth” (7%), “use of forceps/instruments during delivery” (7%) and “prolonged labour and delivery” (4%). As Lalumière et al. (2001) highlighted, the overall level of obstetric problems was low. A scale was created to measure the number of obstetric problems, and this was then correlated with the psychopathy checklist-revised (PCL-R) scores. Interestingly, a higher PCL-R score was negatively related to obstetric problems. This means that those who scored higher on psychopathy were less likely to have experienced problems around the time of birth.

Perinatal and early risk factors were recently examined in a sample of 1,631 children (51.5% girls) from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development, to explore trajectories of psychopathic traits across childhood (Bégin et al., 2021). A few perinatal factors (e.g. psychotropic exposures) and most child (e.g. difficult temperament) and family life factors (e.g. maternal depression, low marital support, socio-economic adversities) significantly increased the odds of a high-stable trajectory of psychopathy or the exacerbation of psychopathic traits across childhood.

Jackson and Beaver (2016) analysed data from the National Longitudinal study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) on the potential effect of breast feeding. Add Health was started almost 30 years ago as a large (n = 90,000) US-based prospective longitudinal study. For their 2016 research, a subsample of participants (n = 2,500) was analysed. Psychopathic traits were measured at Wave 4 and assessed using personality indicators, while breast feeding was measured both dichotomously and as a duration variable. The results indicated that, when controlling for a range of other variables (e.g. age, sex, SES), not having been breastfed or having a short period of breastfeeding were significant predictors of psychopathy, even though the effects were small (Jackson and Beaver, 2016). Interestingly, Jackson and Beaver (2016) also found that low maternal attachment was predictive of later psychopathic traits. This latter finding can be viewed in light of Meloy’s (1988) theory.

According to Meloy (1988), psychopathy involves a poor capacity to bond, that implies a disorder of profound detachment and a disorder of extreme attachment (Meloy, 2002). In other words, the roots of psychopathy might be better understood and assessed by looking at a person’s early experiences with primary attachment figures. Early experiences of childhood rejection from caregivers could lead to aggression and hostility in adulthood (Leary et al., 2006) and are also likely to shape later life outcomes and psychopathic traits (Bowlby, 1952; Barker et al., 2011; Jackson and Beaver, 2016; Kochanska and Kim, 2013; van der Zouwen et al., 2018).

Dutton et al. (1996), studying the characteristics of male abusive personality, found that paternal rejection was the strongest predictor of an abusive personality. Also, Barnow et al. (2002) found that parental rejection played a significant role in the development of ASPD, especially when characteristics such as callousness and aggressiveness were prevalent. Using a large birth cohort of over 4,000 males in Denmark, Raine et al. (1994) found that, among those who experienced maternal rejection in infancy (at Age 1) and birth complications, nearly 9% were involved in violent crime by Age 18, compared to less than 4% of those who had experienced only rejection, birth complications, or neither.

The present study

The aim of this paper is to present new evidence from the CSDD on obstetric and infancy factors and to examine the extent to which factors early in life may have predictive effects on psychopathic traits later in life.

Despite its potential importance for future development, there is a lack of research on obstetric and early infancy factors in relation to later psychopathy (Thomson, 2019). The following research questions will guide the current investigation:

RQ1.

Will obstetric complications predict psychopathic traits at age 48?

RQ2.

Will early infancy conditions and early family factors predict psychopathic traits at age 48?

RQ3.

Will these obstetric complications and early problems have a predictive role depending on the particular cluster of psychopathic traits (Factor 1 versus Factor 2)?

Methods

Sample

The current investigation analyses data from the CSDD. As described elsewhere (Farrington, 2019a, 2021), the CSDD is a prospective longitudinal study of delinquent and criminal behaviour in a community sample of 411 males. The study started in the early 1960s, which explains why some terms used to define some measured variables may sound a little out of date.

The CSDD originally involved a cohort of boys from the registers of six state schools in a working-class area of South London. An office was established for the study in 1961 in this area, and the six state primary schools enlisted in the study were within a one-mile radius of the office. All boys aged 8–9 in those schools were included in the study, so the sample was representative of boys living in the area. There were no private schools in the area. These CSDD men have been followed across the life-course, from age 8 through to age 61 (Farrington et al., 2021; Farrington and Jolliffe, 2022). Most of the CSDD men were Caucasian and from a working-class background and left school by age 16 (61%); their father’s employment was mainly unskilled and semi-skilled manual work in 93.7% of cases, which was higher than the national average at that time of 78.3%. Interviews with the men at age 48 focused on information about their relationships and involved 365 (92.6%) of the 394 men who were still alive (Farrington et al., 2006).

The survey received ethical approval from the Home Office, Cambridge Institute of Criminology and the Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London.

Measures

In total, 18 risk factors, divided into three main categories of life development, were analysed: obstetric complications (i.e. abnormal birth weight, abnormal confinement at birth, obstetric abnormality, severe abnormality of pregnancy) were based on medical records; childhood conditions (i.e. boy’s health, dull mother, feeding problems in infancy, fretful baby, illegitimate child, milestones in infancy, toilet training, unwanted conception, young mother); and family conditions (i.e. father’s and mother’s past and present health, family overcrowding) were based on psychiatric social worker interviews with the parents when the boy was age 8–9.

Obstetric conditions.

Abnormal birth weight was assessed as being very low (below 2.5 kg) or very high (above 4.5 kg). Confinement was considered severely abnormal if more than one of the following complications occurred: asphyxia, bleeding after the onset of labour, caesarean section, foetal distress and forceps delivery. Any obstetric abnormality was recorded if present and assessed. Severe abnormality of pregnancy could include ante-partum haemorrhage (bleeding after 28 weeks of gestation before the onset of labour), cases with albuminuria, severe toxaemia and pre-eclampsia.

Childhood conditions.

Unwanted conception was defined as an undesirable accident in which the mother definitely felt resentful of the pregnancy and possibly attempted to terminate it. The child’s illegitimacy was recorded if the child was born out of wedlock and if the mother and the father were not married to each other at the time of birth registration (Kiernan, 1971).

The psychiatric social workers who visited the homes provided an assessment of the cognitive level of the boy’s mother (e.g. dull mother) and of the general well-being of the boy and his parents. Information about the boy included fretfulness, which described a baby (up to age 1 year) who was unhappy, whined constantly, could not be calmed and was difficult to soothe. The boy’s health was rated as unhealthy if the boy had severe bouts of illness (e.g. bronchitis, asthma) or persistent illness (e.g. otitis). Infant milestones were measured by assessing any form of delay in walking or talking that was classified as a dysfunctional developmental delay (e.g. onset of walking after at 18 months or talking after 2 years). Toilet training was rated as rigid if the determination to get the baby clean by a certain age was influenced by the mother’s fear, anger, or emotionality and if the child was punished for accidents.

Family conditions.

An overcrowded family referred to the number of children and also to the number of rooms available in the house (e.g. fewer rooms than children). The past health of the father and mother was considered unhealthy if significant illnesses, major surgeries or accidents had affected their health. The current poor health of the father and mother was assessed by the psychiatric social worker; chronic or recurrent illness was considered if it was likely that it could cause persistent relapses, periods of illness or disability. (For detailed descriptions of all these childhood risk factors in the CSDD, see West and Farrington, 1973, 1977).

Psychopathy.

Psychopathic traits were assessed as part of the in-person interview at age 48 using the psychopathy checklist: screening version (PCL:SV; Hart et al., 1995), which is the shorter version of the more comprehensive PCL-R (Hare, 2003). The PCL:SV is adapted for use with community samples: it consists of 12 items, each rated on a three-point ordinal scale (0, 1 and 2) for a total score of 24 (Hart et al., 1995). The PCL:SV measures psychopathy based on two factors, which both have scores from 0 to 12. Factor 1 is a measure of psychopathic personality: F1-PP. Factor 2 is a measure of psychopathic behaviour: F2-PB.

The reliability of the PCL:SV, as a measure of psychopathy, is reported elsewhere (Acheson, 2005; see also Auty et al., 2015). In the CSDD, the PCL: SV was administered by a trained forensic psychiatrist (Crystal Romilly) and then independently rescored by a PhD-level psychologist (Simone Ullrich) (Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2021). The total scores of the two diagnostic raters correlated 0.95.

When studying childhood predictors of total psychopathy scores, Farrington and Bergstrøm (2018) noted that these scores were non-linearly related to offending and to violence convictions. Hence, they decided to dichotomise them into high (10 or more) versus low (0–9) PCL:SV scores and found that high scorers were qualitatively different from low scorers. They also dichotomised into high F1-PP (3+) and high F2-PB (5+) scores; dichotomised variables permit an easy and very understandable method of studying how predictors influence outcomes (Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2021).

While psychopathic personality can be found at a subclinical level in the general population (Ene et al., 2022), suggesting that psychopaths are not a distinctive class of individuals (Edens et al., 2006), thresholds can be used to make prevention and treatment recommendations. Establishing thresholds does not deny the dimensional nature of psychopathy, but they may be of practical use. For this investigation, in light of previous studies (Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2018; Zara and Farrington, 2016), the total psychopathy score and Factor 1 (psychopathic personality) and Factor 2 (psychopathic behaviour) scores were dichotomised (please see note on Table 1 for cut off scores).

Analytical strategy

For psychologists and criminologists, individuals are rather more interesting than variables, and the dichotomisation of explanatory variables encourages a focus on the person that encompasses the identification of those individuals who are directly influenced by several risk factors, and who may specifically benefit from specialised early prevention (Farrington and Loeber, 2000).

On a critical note, dichotomisation of continuous variables could have some cost (e.g. losing information, statistical drawbacks), and this should be taken into account before splitting data into two categories (Altman and Royston, 2006). However, variables may be nonlinearly related to outcomes, as research shows (Farrington and Bergstrøm, 2018, 2021), and variables are rarely measured on normally distributed equal-interval scales (like height and weight). Bearing this in mind, dichotomisation makes it possible to compare the predictive strengths of variables, to equalise the sensitivity of their measurement and to better understand which findings can be translated into practice (Agresti, 2007, 2019; Farrington and Loeber, 2000; Hanson, 2022).

In line with past recommendations by Farrington and Loeber (2000), all variables were dichotomised into the “worst” quarter versus the remainder. This cut-off point was decided a priori, and supported by previous research findings using the CSDD data (Farrington, 2019b), and other longitudinal data (Loeber et al., 1999). Dichotomisation did not cause a great loss of information because most variables were measured on 2, 3 or 4 point scales.

Odds ratios (OR) were calculated between the early obstetric complications and family problems and later psychopathic traits. A series of logistic regression analyses were carried out to explore the probability of a specific outcome occurring (e.g. high psychopathic traits) according to whether a set of predictors (e.g. unwanted conception) is statistically important.

Results

Table 1 shows the ORs between the obstetric variables and adult psychopathy at age 48. None of the obstetric problems were predictive of psychopathy, either for the total psychopathy score or for the factor scores. ORs between early childhood conditions and adult psychopathy are also shown on Table 1. Some early childhood problems were significantly predictive of psychopathic traits at age 48. Unwanted conception (by the mother) was significantly (OR = 2.51, p = 0.004) related to high psychopathy. This means that, if the CSDD male was the result of an unwanted pregnancy, this increased his risk of being high on psychopathic traits later in life. At the factor level, unwanted conception was near significance when related to F1 (OR = 1.65, p = 0.09) and significantly related to F2 (OR =1.85, p = 0.03). F1 reflects psychopathic personality traits, and F2 reflects the impulsive lifestyle and antisocial traits of psychopathy.

The likelihood of being an unwanted child was higher for younger mothers (19 years or less) (OR = 1.65, p = 0.06). Moreover, being an unwanted child was significantly more likely when the child was illegitimate (i.e. parents not married) (OR = 4.31, p = 0.001). Being identified as an illegitimate child was also predictive of later total psychopathy (OR = 3.27, p = 0.01), while the relationship was near significance (p = 0.06) for F1 and F2.

Looking at parents’ health, a poor health of the mother at present was significant in predicting total adult psychopathy (OR = 1.98, p = 0.01), and also psychopathic personality (F1) and psychopathic behaviour (F2). On the other hand, the poor health of the father was only near significance in predicting psychopathic behaviour (F2).

All other childhood factors examined (health, being fretful, feeding problems, toilet training and infancy milestones) were not significant in predicting psychopathy.

A series of stepwise logistic regression analyses was carried out to predict high versus low psychopathic traits. All significant variables in Table 1 are included in the analysis as predictors. Unwanted conception (by the mother), mother’s health (at present) and family overcrowding (at ages 8–10) were significant predictors of high psychopathic traits. Table 2 shows all significant results.

Discussion

This paper presents new evidence from the CSDD by examining the extent to which obstetric and early childhood factors predict psychopathic traits later in life (at age 48).

There are several interesting findings from the current investigation. First and foremost, the lack of predictive relationships between obstetric problems and later psychopathy is an interesting finding in itself. It is well-known that null findings are often ignored in published research (Landis et al., 2014; Mlinarić et al., 2017), but this arguably limits the understanding of important psychological phenomena such as psychopathy. The lack of significant relationships is also in line with past literature (Lalumière et al., 2001) and could be viewed as support for what several authors (Glenn et al., 2011; Pullman et al., 2021; Zara et al., 2021) have argued that psychopathy is not actually maladaptive per se. However, the fact that psychopathy can be recognised as an adaptive strategy in response to early life stress and harsh environments (Ene et al., 2022), that is beneficial in dangerous situations (Ellis and Del Giudice, 2019), does not make it a prosocial adaptation.

Consistent with the idea that children with callous-unemotional (CU) behaviours may have difficulty recognising emotional expressions, Bedford et al. (2017) showed in their study of early maternal sensitivity and infants’ mother-directed gaze that infants with low levels of maternal face gaze exhibited higher levels of CU behaviour during face-to-face interactions at age 7, but only when maternal sensitivity was low. Bedford et al. (2017) highlighted the benefit of considering multiple influences in infancy, including maternal and child emotional sensitivity and behaviour, when assessing early predictors of CU behaviours.

The lack of predictive relationships between early childhood characteristics of the boy (such as poor health or rigid toilet training or poor milestones in infancy) and adult psychopathic traits, seems to reinforce the hypothesis, still under scientific scrutiny, that psychopathy does not directly emerge from a dysfunctional development. Without stepping into the debate of whether psychopathy is a disorder or an adaptation (Leedom and Almas, 2012; Zara et al., 2021), psychopathy may instead be a surviving response to the emotional void experienced early in life, as described by Buss et al. (1998), when the child is bereft of parental protection. In other words, psychopathy could be a way of shielding oneself from further rejection and emotional distancing rather than the emerging results of perinatal complications. This may help to understand differences and similarities between affective disturbance and affective deficit (Skeem et al., 2007), even though, as Kerig et al. (2012) advocated, “acquired callousness also might involve deficits in the access to and recognition of internal emotions” (p. 277).

The predictor of unwanted pregnancy (by the mother) is not only interesting per se, but together with family overcrowding, and poor mother’s health seems to describe the parental emotional tension and the social background in which some of the CSDD boys grew up, offering possible explanations for the associations. For example, it could be that these factors are indicators of attachment issues between the mother and the child which are known to be related to psychopathic traits (Bowlby, 1952; Barker et al., 2011; Conradi et al., 2016; Craig et al., 2013; Jackson and Beaver, 2016; Meloy, 1988; Schimmenti et al., 2014). As research shows (Mikulincer et al., 2003), experiences of insensitivity and emotional rejection (i.e. betrayal trauma) may contribute to the process of emotionally distancing oneself from others as a way of reducing or coping with social frustration and emotional disappointment. However, as Kerig et al. (2012) emphasised, the association between betrayal trauma and social difficulty remains largely unexplored.

Furthermore, it is also possible that the fact that the mother was unwell and unwed was associated with stress during pregnancy (Everett and Schechter, 1971), and research indicates that stress during pregnancy is related to later psychopathy of children (Gao et al., 2016). At the time of the birth of the CSDD boys (1953), being pregnant outside wedlock was related to a wide range of other risk factors such as those established by Farrington and Bergstrøm (2018).

A final potential explanation is that growing up in an overcrowded family might not have made it any easy for a child to experience the necessary emotional security, and for the mother to feel an adequate parent, with all the psychological and social consequences for their well-being and their social functioning.

These results together suggest that some early life factors are associated with a higher risk of psychopathic traits, and different pathways could be addressed to prevent the exacerbation of mother’s stress during pregnancy, the dysfunctional bond and emotional detachment between mother and child, and the daily condition within which family life is restrained and compromised. Further examination of the CSDD data needs to be carried out to explore in depth the other developmental emotional, psychological and social conditions of the CSDD men in their adolescence and early adulthood. The key need is to intervene as early as possible to prevent, reduce or avoid serious emotional difficulties and serious problem behaviours later (Allely, 2020).

Limitations

Despite its methodological strengths (Kavish et al., 2020), the CSDD is not immune to limitations. In any longitudinal study, period effects must be considered. The CSDD began at a time (1961) when family and social norms were different from today (Inglehart, 2008). While views on out-of-wedlock births have changed, illegitimacy could carry a sense of rejection today as it did 60 years ago. Despite the social, cultural and historical gap that exists between the beginning and the current development of the CSDD, the present study shows that certain early experiences, such as being unwanted as a child and growing up with an unhealthy mother in an overcrowded family, influence individual development in today’s society as much as they did then.

It would have been important to assess the personality traits of the CSDD parents to examine possible patterns of intergenerational continuity from parents to children. Psychopathic traits were assessed at age 48 for the CSDD men. If CU traits and behaviours had been examined in childhood, and psychopathic traits in adolescence and early adulthood, more information might be available on how these early callous and psychopathic traits may continuously affect the psychological and social functioning of CSDD men.

Conclusions

It is remarkable that childhood factors predict adult psychopathy. These findings show the parental emotional tension and unstable social background in which some of the CSDD men grew up and that predicted psychopathic traits in adulthood (at age 48). The lack of predictive relationships between obstetric problems and later psychopathy suggests that psychopathy may be a way of shielding oneself from further rejection rather than the direct result of perinatal complications.

These findings also emphasise the importance of investigating further the early emotional roots of adult psychopathic traits. Hence, these findings are complex and encouraging. Their complexity is that, consistent with other studies (Sroufe, 2005), they confirm that early attachment experiences are crucial to the development of the person, and that children need to be nurtured and protected to become psychologically and emotionally comfortable adults. Achieving this is obviously difficult when the attachment between child and parent is compromised by many factors, not least from unwanted pregnancy and poor maternal health. However, these findings are encouraging because they suggest that a difference can be made if early intervention prevents emotional detachment from one’s child and significantly enhances the role of parenting.

Acknowledgements

For funding the CSDD, we are very grateful to the Home Office, the Department of Health, the Department for Education, the Rayne Foundation, the Barrow Cadbury Trust, and the Smith-Richardson Foundation. For carrying out criminal record searches, we are very grateful to Gwen Gundry in the 1960s and 1970s, Lynda Morley in the 1980s, Sandra Lambert in the 1990s, Debbie Wilson in the 2000s, Owen Thomas in 2011-12, and Lisa Robinson in 2017.

Early predictors of psychopathy versus PCL:SV scores

Predictors Psychopathic traits OR p F1-PP OR p F2-PB OR p
Low
(n = 236)
High
(n = 68)
Low
(n = 249)
High
(n = 55)
Low
(n = 233)
High
(n = 71)
% % % % % %
Obstetric complications
Low birth weight 5.2 2.2 0.40 n.s. 4.9 0.2.8 0.55 n.s. 5.3 2.1 0.39 n.s.
Abnormal confinement at birth 20.9 17.4 0.80 n.s. 19.6 22.2 1.17 n.s. 21.7 14.9 0.63 n.s.
Obstetric abnormality 52.9 52.2 0.97 n.s. 51.5 58.3 1.32 n.s. 53.9 48.9 0.82 n.s.
Severe abnormality during pregnancy 20.9 21.7 1.05 n.s. 20.2 25.0 1.31 n.s. 21.7 19.1 0.85 n.s.
Childhood conditions
Dull mother 18.9 13.4 0.66 n.s. 19.1 11.3 0.54 n.s. 17.8 17.4 0.974 n.s.
Feeding problems in infancy 28.8 35.0 1.33 n.s. 28.4 38.3 1.56 n.s. 28.7 34.9 1.33 n.s.
Fretful baby 21.6 19.4 0.87 n.s. 21.7 18.4 0.81 n.s. 22.6 15.9 0.65 n.s.
Illegitimate child 3.4 10.3 3.27 0.01 4.0 9.1 2.39 0.06 3.9 8.5 2.30 0.06
Milestones in infancy 16.5 19.4 1.21 n.s. 16.9 18.0 1.08 n.s. 16.2 20.3 1.32 n.s.
Poor health of boy 33.3 29.2 0.83 n.s. 32.8 30.8 0.91 n.s. 32.7 31.3 0.94 n.s.
Toilet training 17.6 16.4 0.92 n.s. 18.7 10.4 0.51 n.s. 18.1 14.5 0.77 n.s.
Unwanted conception (by mother) 16.3 32.8 2.51 0.004 18.4 27.1 1.65 0.09 17.5 28.1 1.85 0.03
Young mother (19 years or less) 21.6 27.9 1.41 n.s. 22.1 27.3 1.32 n.s. 21.0 29.6 1.58 0.07
Family conditions
Family overcrowding (ages 8–9) 16.1 30.8 2.32 0.01 16.9 30.2 2.12 0.01 14.9 34.3 2.99 0.001
Poor health of parents (at present)
Father 21.2 26.2 1.32 n.s. 22.1 23.5 1.09 n.s. 21.2 26.2 1.32 n.s
Mother 24.0 38.5 1.98 0.01 24.2 40.4 2.10 0.01 24.7 35.8 1.71 0.04
Poor health of parents (in past)
Father 27.9 36.1 1.46 n.s. 28.2 36.7 1.48 n.s. 27.4 37.5 1.59 0.06
Mother 28.1 36.9 1.50 0.09 29.9 31.4 1.07 n.s. 28.4 35.8 1.41 n.s.
Notes:

PCL:SV scores for all factors were dichotomised. Factor 1 – Psychopathic personality (F1-PP). Factor 2 – Psychopathic behaviour (F2-PB). High score on PCL:SV = 6 or more. High score on F1-PP = 3 or more. High score on F2-PB = 4 or more. All significant ORs do not overlap the null value (e.g., OR = 1). One-tailed tests were used because of clear directional predictions

Logistic regressions predicting psychopathic traits at age 48

Criterion Predictors B SE(B) Partial OR 95% CI p
High psychopathic traits (vs low psychopathic traits) Unwanted conception (by mother) 0.79 0.35 2.21 1.12–4.35 0.02
Family overcrowding (ages 8–10) 0.72 0.34 2.05 1.05–4.02 0.04
High F1-PP traits (vs Low F1-PP traits) Family overcrowding (ages 8–10) 0.60 0.36 1.83 0.90–3.70 0.09
Mother health (at present) 0.67 0.33 1.95 1.03–3.71 0.04
High F2-PB (vs Low F2-PB) Family overcrowding (ages 8–10) 1.10 0.32 2.99 1.60–5.60 0.001
Notes:

PCL:SV scores for all factors were dichotomised. Factor 1 – Psychopathic personality (F1-PP). Factor 2 – Psychopathic behaviour (F2-PB). High score on PCL:SV = 6 or more. High score on F1-PP = 3 or more. High score on F2-PB = 4 or more. To check the assumption of no multicollinearity (ill conditioning) (Belsley et al., 2004), the variance inflation factor (VIF) and collinearity tolerance (CT) were calculated. Although there are no strict rules about what value of the VIF should cause concern, corresponding guidelines in Menard’s text (1995) suggest that, if the average VIF is not substantially greater than 1, then there is no cause for concern, while tolerance below 0.2 indicates a potential problem (Menard, 1995). In this study, VIF values were as follows: high psychopathic traits vs low psychopathic traits = 1.01; High F1-PP vs Low F1-PP = 1.02; High F2-PB vs Low F2-PB = 1.00. CT values were as follows: high psychopathic traits vs low psychopathic traits = 0.98; High F1-PP vs Low F1-PP = 0.99; High F2-PB vs Low F2-PB = 1.00

References

Acheson, S.K. (2005), “Review of the hare psychopathy checklist-revised”, in Spies, R.A. and Plake, B.S. (Eds), The Sixteenth Mental Measurements Yearbook, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, pp. 429-431.

Agresti, A. (2007), An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis, 2nd ed., Wiley, Hoboken, NJ.

Allely, C.S. (2020), The Psychology of Extreme Violence, Routledge, Abingdon.

Altman, D.G. and Royston, P. (2006), “The cost of dichotomising continuous variables”, BMJ (Clinical Research ed.), Vol. 332 No. 7549, p. 1080.

American Psychiatric Association (2013), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), 5th ed., American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.

Auty, K.M., Farrington, D.P. and Coid, J.W. (2015), “Intergenerational transmission of psychopathy and mediation via psychosocial risk factors”, The British Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 206 No. 1, pp. 26-31, doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.151050.

Barker, E.D., Oliver, B.R., Viding, E., Salekin, R.T. and Maughan, B. (2011), “The impact of prenatal maternal risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on adolescent callous unemotional traits: a 14-year longitudinal investigation”, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 52 No. 8, pp. 878-888, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02397.x.

Barnow, S., Schuckit, M.A., Lucht, M., John, U. and Freyberger, H.J. (2002), “The importance of a positive family history of alcoholism, parental rejection and emotional warmth, behavioral problems and peer substance use for alcohol problems in teenagers: a path analysis”, Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Vol. 63 No. 3, pp. 305-315, doi: 10.15288/jsa.2002.63.305.

Beaver, K.M., Hartman, S. and Belsky, J. (2014), “Differential susceptibility to parental sensitivity based on early-life temperament in the prediction of adolescent affective psychopathic personality traits”, Criminal Justice and Behavior, Vol. 42 No. 5, pp. 546-565, doi: 10.1177%2F0093854814553620.

Beaver, K.M., Vaughn, M.G., DeLisi, M., Barnes, J.C. and Boutwell, B.B. (2012), “The neuropsychological underpinnings to psychopathic personality traits in a nationally representative and longitudinal sample”, Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 83 No. 2, pp. 145-159, doi: 10.1007/s11126-011-9190-2.

Beckett, C. and Taylor, H. (2010), Human Growth and Development, 2nd ed., SAGE, London.

Bedford, R., Wagner, N.J., Rehder, P.D., Propper, C., Willoughby, M.T. and Mills-Koonce, R.W. (2017), “The role of infants’ mother-directed gaze, maternal sensitivity, and emotion recognition in childhood callous unemotional behaviours”, European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Vol. 26 No. 8, pp. 947-956, doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0967-1.

Bégin, V., Fontaine, N.M.G., Vitaro, F., Boivin, M., Tremblay, R.E. and Côté, S.M. (2021), “Perinatal and early-life factors associated with stable and unstable trajectories of psychopathic traits across childhood”, Psychological Medicine, Vol. 53 No. 2, pp. 379-387, doi: 10.1017/S0033291721001586.

Belsley, D.A., Kuh, E. and Welsch, R.E. (2004), Regression Diagnostics: Identifying Influential Data and Sources of Collinearity, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY.

Bergstrøm, H. and Farrington, D.P. (2018), “‘The beat of my heart’: the relationship between resting heart rate and psychopathy in a prospective longitudinal study”, Journal of Criminal Psychology, Vol. 8 No. 4, pp. 333-344, doi: 10.1108/JCP-07-2018-0029.

Blonigen, D.M., Hicks, B.M., Krueger, R.F., Patrick, C.J. and Iacono, W.G. (2006), “Continuity and change in psychopathic traits as measured via normal-range personality: a longitudinal-biometric study”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 115 No. 1, pp. 85-95, doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.85.

Boduszek, D. and Debowska, A. (2016), “Critical evaluation of psychopathy measurement (PCL-R and SRP-III/SF) and recommendations for future research”, Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 44, pp. 1-12, doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2015.11.004.

Boduszek, D., Debowska, A., Sherretts, N., Willmott, D., Boulton, M., Kielkiewicz, K. and Hyland, P. (2021), “Are prisoners more psychopathic than non-forensic populations? Profiling psychopathic traits among prisoners, community adults, university students, and adolescents”, Deviant Behavior, Vol. 42 No. 2, pp. 232-244, doi: 10.1080/01639625.2019.1665.

Boduszek, D., Debowska, A., Willmott, D., Jones, A.D., DeLisi, M. and Kirkman, G. (2019), “Is female psychopathy linked with child abuse? An empirical investigation using a person-centered approach”, Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, Vol. 28 No. 6, pp. 708-725, doi: 10.1080/10538712.2019.1592.

Bowlby, J. (1969), Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1, Basic Books, New York, NY.

Boyle, M.H., Miskovic, V., Van Lieshout, R., Duncan, L., Schmidt, L.A., Hoult, L., Paneth, N. and Saigal, S. (2011), “Psychopathology in young adults born at extremely low birth weight”, Psychological Medicine, Vol. 41 No. 8, pp. 1763-1774, doi: 10.1017/s0033291710002357.

Brandon, A.R., Pitts, S., Wayne, H., Denton, W.H., Stringer, C.H. and Evans, H.M. (2009), “A history of the theory of prenatal attachment”, Journal of Prenatal and Perinatal Psychology and Health, Vol. 23 No. 4, pp. 201-222.

Bretherton, I. and Waters, E. (1985), “Growing points of attachment theory and research. Attachment theory: retrospect and prospect”, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 50 Nos 1/2, pp. 3-35.

Buss, D.M., Haselton, M.G., Shackelford, T.K., Bleske, A.L. and Wakefield, J.C. (1998), “Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels”, American Psychologist, Vol. 53 No. 5, pp. 533-548, doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.5.533.

Condon, J.T. and Corkindale, C. (1997), “The correlates of antenatal attachment in pregnant women”, British Journal of Medical Psychology, Vol. 70 No. 4, pp. 359-372.

Conradi, H.J., Boertien, S.D., Cavus, H. and Verschuere, B. (2016), “Examining psychopathy from an attachment perspective: the role of fear of rejection and abandonment”, The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, Vol. 27 No. 1, pp. 92-109, doi: 1080/14789949.2015.1077264.

Craig, R.L., Gray, N.S. and Snowden, R.J. (2013), “Recalled parental bonding, current attachment, and the triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 55 No. 4, pp. 345-350, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.012.

Cranley, M.S. (1981), “Roots of attachment: the relationship of parents with their unborn”, Birth Defects: Original Article Series, Vol. 17, pp. 59-83, doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1997.tb01912.x.

De Brito, S.A., Forth, A.E., Baskin-Sommers, A.R., Brazil, I.A., Kimonis, E.R., Pardini, D., Frick, P.J., Blair, R.J.R. and Viding, E. (2021), “Psychopathy”, Nature Reviews Disease Primers, Vol. 7, pp. 1-49, doi: 10.1038/s41572-021-00282-1.

Dekleyn, M. and Greenberg, M. (2008), “Attachment and psychopathology in childhood”, in Cassidy J. and Shaver, P. (Eds), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed., Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp. 637-665.

Donohue, J. and Levitt, S. (2001), “The impact of legalized abortion on crime”, Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 116 No. 2, pp. 379-420.

Dutton, D.G., Starzomski, A. and Ryan, L. (1996), “Antecedents of abusive personality and abusive behavior in wife assaulters”, Journal of Family Violence, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 113-132.

Edens, J.F., Marcus, D.K., Lilienfeld, S.O. and Poythress, N.G., Jr. (2006), “Psychopathic, not psychopath: taxometric evidence for the dimensional structure of psychopathy”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 115 No. 1, pp. 131-144, doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.131.

Ellis, B.J. and Del Giudice, M. (2019), “Developmental adaptation to stress: an evolutionary perspective”, Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 70 No. 1, pp. 111-139, doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011732.

Ene, I., Wong, K.K.-Y. and Salali, G.D. (2022), “Is it good to be bad? An evolutionary analysis of the adaptive potential of psychopathic traits”, Evolutionary Human Sciences, Vol. 4 No. e37, pp. 1-24, doi: 10.1017/ehs.2022.36.

Everett, R.B. and Schechter, M.D. (1971), “A comparative study of prenatal anxiety in the unwed mother”, Child Psychiatry and Human Development, Vol. 2 No. 2, pp. 84-91, doi: 10.1007/BF01434641.

Farrington, D.P. (Ed.) (2005), “Integrated developmental and life-course theories of offending”, Advances in Criminological Theory, Vol. 14, Routledge, New York, NY.

Farrington, D.P. (2019a), “The Cambridge study in delinquent development”, in Eaves, D, Webster, C.D., Haque, Q. and Eaves-Thalken, J. (Eds), Risk Rules: A Practical Guide to Structured Professional Judgment and Violence Prevention, Pavilion Publishing, Howe, East Sussex, pp. 225-233.

Farrington, D.P. (2019b), “The development of violence from age 8 to 61”, Aggressive Behavior, Vol. 45 No. 4, pp. 365-376, doi: 10.1002/ab.21831.

Farrington, D.P. (2021), “New findings in the Cambridge study in delinquent development”, in Barnes, J.C. and Forde, D.R. (Eds), The Encyclopedia of Research Methods in Criminology and Criminal Justice, Vol. 1, Wiley-Blackwell, Hoboken, NJ, pp. 96-103.

Farrington, D.P. and Bergstrøm, H. (2018), “Family background and psychopathy”, Patrick, C.J. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychopathy, 2nd ed., Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp. 354-379.

Farrington, D.P. and Bergstrøm, H. (2021), “High resting heart rate protects against childhood risk factors in predicting adult psychopathy”, Journal of Criminal Psychology, Vol. 11 No. 2, pp. 141-156, doi: 10.1108/JCP-01-2021-0003.

Farrington, D.P. and Bergstrøm, H. (2022), “The development of psychopathy through the lifespan and its relation to offending”, in Marques, P.B., Paulino, M. and Alho, L. (Eds), Psychopathy and Criminal Behavior. Current Trends and Challenges, Academic Press, Oxford, pp. 105-126, doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-811419-3.00014-5.

Farrington, D.P. and Jolliffe, D. (2022), “Latest results from the Cambridge study in delinquent development (CSDD)”, in Andersson, R.R. and Wahlgren, P. (Eds), A Life in Criminology: Festschrift to Jerzy Sarnecki, Justus Forlag, Uppsala, pp. 45-55.

Farrington, D.P. and Loeber, R. (2000), “Some benefits of dichotomization in psychiatric and criminological research”, Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, Vol. 10 No. 2, pp. 100-122, doi: 10.1002/cbm.349.

Farrington, D.P., Coid, J.W., Harnett, L., Jolliffe, D., Soteriou, N., Turner, R. and West, D.J. (2006), Criminal Careers up to Age 50 and Life Success up to Age 48: New Findings from the Cambridge Study in Delinquent Development, Research Study No. 299, Home Office, London.

Farrington, D.P., Jolliffe, D. and Coid, J.W. (2021), “Cohort profile: the Cambridge study in delinquent development (CSDD)”, Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 278-291, doi: 10.1007/s40865-021-00162-y.

Gao, Y., Huang, Y. and Xiaobo Li, X. (2017), “Interaction between prenatal maternal stress and autonomic arousal in predicting conduct problems and psychopathic traits in children”, Journal of Psychopathology Behavior Assessment, Vol. 39 No. 1, pp. 1-14, doi: 10.1007/s10862-016-9556-8.

Glenn, A. (2018), “Early life predictors of callous-unemotional and psychopathic traits”, Infant Mental Health Journal, Vol. 40 No. 1, pp. 39-53, doi: 10.1002/imhj.21757.

Glenn, A.L., Kurzban, R. and Raine, A. (2011), “Evolutionary theory and psychopathy”, Aggression and Violent Behavior, Vol. 16 No. 5, pp. 371-380, doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009.

Glenn, A.L., Raine, A., Venables, P.H. and Mednick, S. (2007), “Early temperamental and psychophysiological precursors of adult psychopathic personality”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 115 No. 3, pp. 508-518, doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.508.

Gurol, A. and Palot, S. (2012), “The effects of baby massage on attachment between mother and their infants”, Asian Nursing Research, Vol. Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 35-41, doi: 10.1016/j.anr.2012.02.006.

Hanson, R.K. (2022), Prediction Statistics for Psychological Assessment, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.

Hare, R.D. (2003), Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R), 2nd ed., Multi-Health Systems, Toronto, ON.

Hare, R.D. and Neumann, C.S. (2009), “Psychopathy: assessment and forensic implications”, The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 54 No. 12, pp. 791-802, doi: 10.1177/070674370905401202.

Hart, S.D., Cox, D.N. and Hare, R.D. (1995), Hare Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV), Multi-Health Systems, Toronto, ON.

Inglehart, R.F. (2008), “Changing values among western publics from 1970 to 2006”, West European Politics, Vol. 31 Nos 1/2, pp. 130-146.

Ishikawa, S.S. and Raine, A. (2003), “Prefrontal deficits and antisocial behavior: a causal model”, Lahey, B.B., Moffitt, T.E. and Caspi, A. (Eds), Causes of Conduct Disorder and Juvenile Delinquency, The Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp. 277-304.

Jackson, D.B. and Beaver, K.M. (2016), “The association between breastfeeding exposure and duration, neuropsychological deficits, and psychopathic personality traits in offspring: the moderating role of 5HTTLPR”, Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 87 No. 1, pp. 107-127, doi: 10.1007/s11126-015-9366-2.

Kavish, N., Bergstrøm, H., Piquero, A.R., Farrington, D.P. and Boutwell, B.B. (2020), “The longitudinal association between resting heart rate and psychopathic traits from a normative personality perspective”, American Journal of Criminal Justice, Vol. 45 No. 3, pp. 410-425, doi: 10.1007/s12103-019-09504-5.

Kerig, P.K., Bennett, D.C., Thompson, M., Stephen, P. and Becker, S.P. (2012), “‘Nothing really matters’: emotional numbing as a link between trauma exposure and callousness in delinquent”, Youth, Journal of Traumatic Stress, Vol. 25, pp. 272-279, doi: 10.1002/jts.21700.

Kiernan, K.E. (1971), “The illegitimacy phenomenon of England and Wales in the 1950’ and 1960’s”, available at: www.york.ac.uk/media/spsw/documents/research-and-publications/Kiernan_1971_IllegitimacyPhenomenonOfEnglandAndWales.pdf

Kochanska, G. and Kim, S. (2013), “Early attachment organization with both parents and future behavior problems: from infancy to middle childhood”, Child Development, Vol. 84 No. 1, pp. 283-296, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x.

Lalumière, M.L., Harris, G.T. and Rice, M.E. (2001), “Developmental instability and psychopathy”, Evolution and Human Behavior, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 75-92, doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00064-7.

Landis, R.S., James, L.R., Lance, C.E., Pierce, C.E. and Rogelberg, S.G. (2014), “When is nothing something? Editorial for the null results special issue of Journal of Business and Psychology”, Journal of Business and Psychology, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 163-167, doi: 10.1007/s10869-014-9347-8.

Leary, M.R., Jean, M., Twenge, J.M. and Quinlivan, E. (2006), “Interpersonal rejection as a determinant of anger and aggression”, Personality and Social Psychology Review, Vol. 10 No. 2, pp. 111-132, doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_2.

Leedom, L.J. and Almas, H.L. (2012), “Is psychopathy a disorder or an adaptation?”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 3, p. 549, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00549.

Lewis, A. (1974), “Psychopathic personality: a most elusive category”, Psychological Medicine, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 133-140, doi: 10.1017/s0033291700041969.

Loeber, R., Farrington, D.P., Stouthamer-Loeber, M. and van Kammen, W.B. (1998), Antisocial Behavior and Mental Health Problems, Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.

Loeber, R., Wei, E., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., Huizinga, D. and Thornberry, T. (1999), “Behavioral antecedents to serious and violent juvenile offending: joint analyses from the Denver youth survey, Pittsburgh youth study, and the Rochester development study”, Studies on Crime and Crime Prevention, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 245-263.

Lynam, D.R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T.E., Loeber, R. and Magda Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2007), “Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 116 No. 1, pp. 155-165, doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.155.

Mack, T.D., Hackney, A.A. and Pyle, M. (2011), “The relationship between psychopathic traits and attachment behavior in a non-clinical population”, Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 51 No. 5, pp. 584-588, doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.019.

Meloy, J.R. (1988), The Psychopathic Mind: Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment, Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Lanham, MD.

Meloy, J.R. (2002), Violent Attachments, Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, Lanham, MD.

Menard, S. (1995), Applied Logistic Regression Analysis, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.

Mlinarić, A., Horvat, M. and Šupak Smolčić, V. (2017), “Dealing with the positive publication bias: why you should really publish your negative results”, Biochemia Medica, Vol. 27 No. 3, doi: 10.11613/BM.2017.030201.

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P.R. and Pereg, D. (2003), “Attachment theory and affect regulation: the dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related strategies”, Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 77-102, doi: 10.1023/A:1024515519160.

Olds, D., Henderson, C.R., Jr., Cole, R., Eckenrode, J., Kitzman, H., Luckey, D., Pettitt, L., Sidora, K., Morris, P. and Powers, J. (1998), “Long-term effects of nurse home visitation on children’s criminal and antisocial behavior: 15-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial”, JAMA, Vol. 280 No. 14, pp. 1238-1244, doi: 10.1001/jama.280.14.1238.

Pullman, L.E., Refaie, N., Lalumière, M.L. and Krupp, D.B. (2021), “Is psychopathy a mental disorder or an adaptation? Evidence from a meta-analysis of the association between psychopathy and handedness”, Evolutionary Psychology, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 1-17, doi: 10.1177/14747049211040447.

Raine, A., Brennan, P. and Mednick, S.A. (1994), “Birth complications combined with early maternal rejection at age 1 year predispose to violent crime at age 18 years”, Archives of General Psychiatry, Vol. 51 No. 12, pp. 984-988, doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950120056009.

Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Pace, U., Manzella, S., Di Carlo, G. and Caretti, V. (2014), “The relationship between attachment and psychopathy: a study with a sample of violent offenders”, Current Psychology, Vol. 33 No. 3, pp. 256-270, doi: 10.1007/s12144-014-9211-z.

Shoesmith, G.L. (2015), “Crime, teenage abortion, and unwantedness”, Crime & Delinquency, Vol. 63 No. 11, pp. 1458-1490, doi: 10.1177/0011128715615882.

Skeem, J.L. and Cooke, D.J. (2010), “Is criminal behavior a central component of psychopathy? Conceptual directions for resolving the debate”, Psychological Assessment, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 433-445, doi: 10.1037/a0008512.

Skeem, J., Johansson, P., Andershed, H., Kerr, M. and Eno Louden, J. (2007), “Two subtypes of psychopathic violent offenders that parallel primary and secondary variants”, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 116 No. 2, pp. 395-409, doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.395.

Sroufe, L. (2005), “Attachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood”, Attachment & Human Development, Vol. 7 No. 4, pp. 349-367, doi: 10.1080/14616730500365928.

Thomson, N.D. (2019), Understanding Psychopathy: The Biopsychosocial Perspective, Routledge, Abingdon, doi: 10.4324/9780203703304.

Tibbetts, S. and Piquero, A. (1999), “The influence of gender, low birth weight, and disadvantaged environment in predicting early onset of offending: a test of Moffitt’s interaction hypothesis”, Criminology, Vol. 37 No. 4, pp. 843-877, doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.1999.tb00507.x.

Van den Bergh, B.R., Mulder, E.J.H., Mennes, M. and Glover, V. (2005), “Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review”, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 237-258.

van der Zouwen, M., Hoeve, M., Hendriks, A.M., Asscher, J.J. and Stams, G.J.J.M. (2018), “The association between attachment and psychopathic traits”, Aggression and Violent Behavior, Vol. 43, pp. 45-55, doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2018.09.002.

Vaughn, B.E., Bost, K.K. and van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2008), “Attachment and temperament: Additive and interactive influences on behavior, affect, and cognition during infancy and childhood”, in Cassidy, J. and Shaver, P.R. (Eds), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed., Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp. 192-216.

Vitale, J.E. (2022), “Introduction to psychopathy”, in Vitale, J.E. (Ed.), The Complexity pf Psychopathy, Springer, Cham, pp. 1-4.

West, D.J. and Farrington, D.P. (1973), Who Becomes Delinquent? Heinemann, London.

West, D.J. and Farrington, D.P. (1977), The Delinquent Way of Life, Heinemann, London.

Zara, G. and Farrington, D.P. (2016), Criminal Recidivism: Explanation, Prediction and Prevention, Routledge, Abingdon.

Zara, G., Bergstrøm, H. and Farrington, D.P. (2021), “The sexual life of men with psychopathic traits”, Journal of Criminological Research, Policy and Practice, Vol. 7 No. 2, pp. 164-178, doi: 10.1108/JCRPP-04-2020-0036.

Further reading

Feldman, J. (2007), “The effect of support expectations on prenatal attachment: an evidence based approach for intervention on an adolescent population”, Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, Vol. 24 No. 3, pp. 209-234.

Acknowledgements

Conflict of interest: The authors of this article declare no conflict of interest.Financial support: No funding was received to carry out this study.

Corresponding author

Georgia Zara can be contacted at: georgia.zara@unito.it

About the authors

Georgia Zara is based at the Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy.

Henriette Bergstrøm is based at the School of Psychology, College of Health, Psychology and Social Care, University of Derby, Derby, UK.

David P. Farrington is based at the Institute of Criminology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.

Related articles